首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   85篇
  免费   4篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   1篇
  2018年   1篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   4篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   10篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   2篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   1篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   2篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   1篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   4篇
  1982年   4篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
  1979年   1篇
  1977年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   3篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   7篇
  1968年   1篇
  1954年   1篇
排序方式: 共有89条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
51.
52.
Book reviews     
Institutions represent both the conscious and unconscious aspects of the interacting members. Psychotherapists need to address their unconscious position in the institution as well as the conscious aspects of their work. This paper describes the possible relevance that psychotherapy may have at the unconscious level. The notion of the ‘social defence system’ is extremely useful in finding one's way in the engulfing emotional attitudes, and for tolerating the often dismaying sense of irrelevance that psychotherapy appears to attract. The social defence system of a prison and a mental hospital are considered in this light and some thought are devoted to the changing state of the National Health Service at present.  相似文献   
53.
Complex decision making typically involves many agents attempting to aggregate many alternatives. If agents' preferences are unconstrained, cyclic outcomes are highly probable. In contrast, we show that if agents share similar models of the choice domain, then a stable collective outcome occurs with about 90% probability. These results have implications for systems of interacting agents, such as group decisions by individuals in social settings or interpretations of sense data by competing perceptual modules of a brain.  相似文献   
54.
CALLERGÅRD  ROBERT 《Synthese》1999,120(1):19-26
My object is to question a recurrent claim made to the point that Thomas Reid (1710–1796) was hostile to ether theories and that this hostility had its source in his distinctive interpretation of the first of Newton's regulæ philosophandi. Against this view I will argue that Reid did not have any quarrel at all with unobservable or theoretical entities as such, and that his objections against actual theories concerning ether were scientific rather than philosophical, even when based on Newton's first rule. I argue further that Reid's insistence on Newton's rule concerns, not direct observation, but rather the notion of explanation itself.  相似文献   
55.
When a shape moves behind a very narrow window, or “slit,” the entire shape can be recognized, although at any instant only meaningless fragments are visible. In some manner, the human visual system has the ability to piece together these sequential fragments into a coherent shape that is seen as moving across the largely occluded field. As a first step toward understanding how this feat may be accomplished, we show some minimal conditions for recovering the direction and speed of motion of an unknown shape moving behind a slit. Two important conditions are that the object be rigid and that its motion be a simple transition in the image plane.  相似文献   
56.
57.
Scaling techniques were employed to establish the relation between perceived distance ratio and physical distance ratio. Measurements were made both with and without free eye movement and under two states of convergence. The results were confirmed using a matching technique. With free eye movement, the perceived ratio is a monotonic increasing function of the physical ratio. Without eye movement, the perceived ratio generally increases, then decreases, as the physical ratio increases. For a given physical ratio, perceived distance ratio is less in the absence of voluntary eye movements. Convergence produces depth micropsia when eye movements are permitted, but not in their absence.  相似文献   
58.
Positive reinforcement, physical guidance, and fading procedures were used to teach two severely retarded children motor responses to a variety of verbal instructions. Subjects' responses to one set of instructions provided the focus for the training procedures. Their responses to a second set of instructions were used to assess the generalized effects of training. The frequency of responses to both sets of instructions was evaluated during Baseline 1, Training 1, Baseline 2, and Training 2 periods. During the training periods, this evaluation was made after the daily training sessions when no training procedures were in effect. Results indicated that the subjects showed pronounced increases in instruction-following behaviors (both trained and untrained) during training periods with decreases in such behavior occurring during the Baseline 2 period. The general findings demonstrate the applicability of the training procedures for producing and maintaining instruction-following behaviors in severely retarded children and for facilitating appropriate responding to instructions not directly involved in training.  相似文献   
59.
The present study examined the effectiveness of a group language training procedure for directly increasing and generalizing the rate of verbal interaction among four elderly, socially isolated, moderately mentally retarded men. A withdrawal of treatment design was used to examine the effect of the procedure that used verbal prompts, behavioral rehearsal, and contingent social praise. Changes in behavior were examined in two generalization settings, one similiar to the training environment (Generalization I) and the other arranged as part of the subjects' daily routine (Generalization II). Baseline data indicated no verbal interaction among the subjects. During treatment the training procedure increased the rate of subjects' verbal interactions not only in the training situation, but also in the two generalization settings. An analysis of the data obtained during the Generalization II situation indicated that subjects' verbal interaction increased not only among themselves, but with nonsubject peers present in this setting. Follow-up data showed that increases in rates of verbal interaction were maintained four months after the cessation of training. The implications of the results for program generalization and work with the language deficient individual is discussed.  相似文献   
60.
A compelling question in teratology research is whether the experimental model used allows for extrapolation from laboratory animals to man. Our thesis is that behaviors that are biologically relevant for the animal are more likely to reveal the relationship between the behavior and its substrate than are non-specific behaviors. Applying this reasoning to the study of the ontogenesis of the behavior, we plead for including the use of species-typical behaviors in testing for teratogenic effects. Our further discussion is focussed upon the different levels at which an analysis of teratogenic effects may be carried out. At one level, the analysis is directed towards the behavioral effects of a given treatment; at another level to specify the function that is impaired by the treatment.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号