全文获取类型
收费全文 | 61篇 |
免费 | 7篇 |
专业分类
68篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 3篇 |
2017年 | 3篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 1篇 |
2013年 | 7篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 2篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 2篇 |
2007年 | 5篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有68条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Gordon L. Flett Paul L. Hewitt Teresa Whelan Thomas R. Martin 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2007,25(4):255-277
Two studies were conducted to examine the psychometric properties and correlates of the Perfectionism Cognitions Inventory
(PCI) when administered to clinical samples. The PCI is a 25-item measure of automatic thoughts with themes involving perfectionism
and beliefs that perfection should be attained. Analyses indicated that the PCI is unidimensional and it has adequate internal
consistency. Correlational analyses confirmed that the PCI is associated with psychological distress and deficits in cognitive
self-management, including lack of self-reinforcement, lack of a positive self-focus, and perfectionistic inflexibility. Moreover,
tests of incremental validity revealed that the PCI accounts for unique variance in levels of anxiety and depression symptoms
after removing variance attributable to trait perfectionism dimensions. Overall, the findings suggest that automatic thoughts
involving perfectionistic themes can be assessed in a reliable and valid manner in clinical samples. Moreover, it is important
to assess perfectionism cognitions as a supplement to trait perfectionism measures when evaluating the role of perfectionism
in psychological distress and associated deficits in cognitive self-control.
This research was supported by major research grants from Health Canada and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
(SSHRC) of Canada to the first two authors, and a Canada Research Chair in Personality & Health awarded to the first author.
The fourth author, Thomas Martin, is now deceased and this paper is dedicated to his memory. 相似文献
12.
The present study considered the relation between adolescent gambling behavior and the perceived environment, the component
of Jessor and Jessor’s (1977) Problem Behavior Theory that assesses the ways that adolescents perceive the attitudes and behaviors
of parents and peers. The predominantly African-American sample included 188 sophomores from two urban public high schools.
Using the South Oaks Gambling Screen-Revised for Adolescents to assess gambling risk, rates of both at-risk (20.7%) and problem
(12.8%) gambling were found to be high. Boys displayed more gambling problems than did girls. The perceived environment accounted
for significant variance in gambling problems and frequency, with proximal components displaying stronger relationships than
distal components. Perceiving parent gambling and friend models for problem behavior were positively correlated with gambling
problems, and friend models were positively related to gambling frequency. Among girls, family support was positively related
to gambling problems. Among boys, this relation was negative.
The authors Emerson M. Wickwire, Jr. James P. Whelan, and Andrew W. Meyers are members of The Institute for Gambling Education
and Research, Department of Psychology, The University of Memphis, Memphis, TN. The primary research interest of this group
is the prevention and treatment of gambling related problems.
David M. Murray, Ph.D., is the Chair of the Division of Epidemiology at the School of Public Health, The Ohio State University,
Columbus, OH. 相似文献
13.
Nick Haslam Brock Bastian Christopher Fox Jennifer Whelan 《Personality and individual differences》2007,42(8):1621-1631
Lay conceptions of personality change and continuity were examined in a sample of 112 undergraduates. Participants rated their personal change over 5 years (past or future), the change they perceived to be normative over 10-year age spans between 15 and 65, their beliefs about whether personality is fixed or malleable (“lay theories”) and their beliefs about the causes of personality change and continuity. Beliefs about normative personality change generally corresponded to research evidence on adult trajectories of the Big Five factors, with some age bias, whereas recalled and anticipated personal change tended to be more positive than these norms. Participants tended to endorse environmental causes more for personality change than for continuity. Lay theories were not consistently associated with these causal beliefs, or with beliefs about personal and normative change. 相似文献
14.
15.
Several single studies concerned with work training with the mentally handicapped have been reported in the literature (Bitter & Bolanovich, 1966; Huddle, 1967; Gold, 1969; Screven, Straka & Lafond, 1971). Most have been concerned with issues of acquisition and motivation to perform. No studies have been reported which focus on the evaluation of different techniques of training or which assess rate of acquisition against that of non-handicapped controls. The aim of this experiment is twofold. It uses a complex task, analysed by MTM-2, to enable comparison between the performances of mentally handicapped adults and adults from a 'normal' population, in the acquisition of new work skills. Various strategies of training were compared and the increase in the rate and quality of performance following acquisition was measured for both, in terms of speed and accuracy of production. Results add further support for the notion that the potential of mentally handicapped individuals is commonly underestimated. It is hoped that the findings provide a basis from which further experiments can be developed and evaluated. 相似文献
16.
17.
The Psychological Record - Most analyses of reaction time (RT) data are conducted by using the statistical techniques with which psychologists are most familiar, such as analysis of variance on the... 相似文献
18.
Dymond S Roche B Forsyth JP Whelan R Rhoden J 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2007,88(2):249-262
Research on the emergence of human avoidance behavior in the absence of direct contact with an aversive event is somewhat limited. Consistent with work on derived relational responding, the present study sought to investigate the transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with the relational frames of Same and Opposite. Participants were first exposed to nonarbitrary and arbitrary relational training and testing in order to establish Same and Opposite relations among arbitrary stimuli. The training tasks were; Same-A1-B1, Same-A1-C1, Opposite-A1-B2, Opposite-A1-C2. Next, all possible combinatorially entailed (i.e., B-C and C-B) relations were tested. During the avoidance-conditioning phase, one stimulus (B1) from the relational network signaled a simple avoidance response that cancelled a scheduled presentation of an aversive image and sound. All but one of the participants who met the criteria for conditioned avoidance also demonstrated derived avoidance by emitting the avoidance response in the presence of C1 and the nonavoidance response in the presence of C2. Control participants who were not exposed to relational training and testing did not show derived avoidance. Implications of the findings for understanding clinically significant avoidance behavior are discussed. 相似文献
19.
20.
Simon Dymond Michael W. Schlund Bryan Roche Robert Whelan Jennifer Richards Cara Davies 《Behaviour research and therapy》2011,49(10):614-621
Symbolic generalization of avoidance may underlie the aetiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. The aim of the present study was to demonstrate inferred threat-avoidance and safety (non-avoidance) behaviours that occur in the presence of stimuli indirectly related to learned threat and safety cues. A laboratory experiment was conducted involving two symbolic stimulus equivalence relations consisting of three physically dissimilar stimuli (avoidance cues: AV1–AV2–AV3 and neutral cues: N1–N2–N3). During avoidance learning involving aversive images and sounds, a key-press avoidance response was trained for one member of one of the relations (AV2) and non-avoidance for another (N2). Inferred threat and safety behaviour and ratings of the likelihood of aversive events were tested with presentations of all remaining stimuli. Findings showed a significantly high percentage of avoidance to both the learned and inferred threat cues and less avoidance to both the learned and inferred safety cues. Ratings in the absence of avoidance were high during training and testing to threat cues and low to safety cues and were generally lower in the presence of avoidance. Implications for associative and behavioural accounts of avoidance, and modern therapies for anxiety disorders are discussed. 相似文献