全文获取类型
收费全文 | 87篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
92篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 1篇 |
2017年 | 2篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2015年 | 1篇 |
2013年 | 3篇 |
2012年 | 1篇 |
2011年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 4篇 |
2007年 | 2篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 1篇 |
2004年 | 2篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 2篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有92条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Recent accounts of the pathomechanism underlying apraxia of speech (AOS) were based on the speech production model of Levelt, Roelofs, and Meyer, and Meyer (1999)1999. The apraxic impairment was localized to the phonetic encoding level where the model postulates a mental store of motor programs for high-frequency syllables. Varley and Whiteside (2001a) assumed that in patients with AOS syllabic motor programs are no longer accessible and that these patients are required to use a subsyllabic encoding route. In this study, we tested this hypothesis by exploring the influence of syllable frequency and syllable structure on word repetition in 10 patients with AOS. A significant effect of syllable frequency on error rates was found. Moreover, apraxic errors on consonant clusters were influenced by their position relative to syllable boundaries. These results demonstrate that apraxic patients have access to the syllabary, but that they fail to retrieve the syllabic motor patterns correctly. Our findings are incompatible with a subsyllabic route model of apraxia of speech. 相似文献
72.
73.
Wolfram Boucsein Florian Schaefer Evgeni N. Sokolov Christina Schröder John J. Furedy 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2001,36(2):137-153
A framework for accounting for emotional phenomena proposed by Sokolov and Boucsein (2000) employs conceptual dimensions that
parallel those of hue, brightness, and saturation in color vision. The approach that employs the concepts of emotional quality,
intensity, and saturation has been supported by psychophysical emotional scaling data gathered from a few trained observers.
We report cortical evoked potential data obtained during the change between different emotions expressed in schematic faces.
Twenty-five subjects (13 male, 12 female) were presented with a positive, a negative, and a neutral computer-generated face
with random interstimulus intervals in a within-subjects design, together with four meaningful and four meaningless control
stimuli made up from the same elements. Frontal, central, parietal, and temporal ERPs were recorded from each hemisphere.
Statistically significant outcomes in the P300 and N200 range support the potential fruitfulness of the proposed color-vision-model-based
approach to human emotional space. 相似文献
74.
Wolfram Hinzen 《Erkenntnis》2000,52(3):281-311
I argue that the implementation of theDummettian program of an ``anti-realist' semanticsrequires quite different conceptions of the technicalmeaning-theoretic terms used than those presupposed byDummett. Starting from obvious incoherences in anattempt to conceive truth conditions as assertibilityconditions, I argue that for anti-realist purposesnon-epistemic semantic notions are more usefully kept apart from epistemic ones rather than beingreduced to them. Embedding an anti-realist theory ofmeaning in Martin-Löf's Intuitionistic Type Theory(ITT) takes care, however, of many notorious problemsthat have arisen in trying to specify suitableintuitionistic notions of semantic value,truth-conditions, and validity, taking into accountthe so-called ``defeasibility of evidence' forassertions in empirical discourses. 相似文献
75.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
76.
Evgeni N. Sokolov Wolfram Boucsein 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2000,35(2):81-119
Despite a wide variety of emotions that can be subjectively experienced, the emotion space has consistently revealed a low
dimensionality. The search for corresponding somato-visceral response patterns has been only moderately successful. The authors
suggest a solution based on an assumed parallelism between emotion coding and color coding. According to the color detection
model proposed by Sokolov and co-workers, neurons responsible for color detection are triggered by a combination of excitations
in a limited number of input cells. Similarly, a limited number of input channels may feed complex emotion detectors being
located on a hypersphere in a four-dimensional emotion space, the three angles of which correspond to emotional tone, intensity,
and saturation, in parallel to hue, lightness, and saturation in color perception. The existence of such a four-dimensional
emotion space in the subjective domain is shown by using schematic facial expressions as stimuli.
A neurophysiological model is provided in which reticular, hypothalamic, and limbic structures constitute input channels of
an emotion detecting system, thus acting as the first layer of emotion predetectors. Hypothalamic neurons with differential
sensitivity for various transmitters may elicit a subsequent selective activation in a second layer of predetectors at the
thalamic level. The latter are suggested to trigger emotion detectors located in cortical areas, the action of which should
be revealed by measures of central nervous system activity. Preliminary results from evoked potential studies show that switching
between schematic faces that express different emotions may be used as an objective measure for establishing a psychophysiological
emotion space. 相似文献
77.
John J. Furedy Ph.D. Boris Damke Wolfram Boucsein 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2000,35(1):17-34
Numerous studies have indicated that, consistent with current “cognitive” accounts of information processing, human Pavlovian
autonomic discrimination acquisition cannot occur without awareness of the CS-US relationship. However, extinction studies
have suggested that awareness is not necessary, findings that, in information-processing terms, have been explained by assuming
that the processing by the extinction stage is parallel (automatic) rather than serial (controlled). This explanation was
tested in an 80-subject study. The first, acquisition phase was a standard semantic differential conditioning arrangement
with a 96-db white noise as US, and a “long” CS-US interval of 8 s, with ten trials each of CS+ (paired with US) and CS− (unpaired)
trials. In extinction (USs omitted), in order to obtain non-autonomic indices of processing and thereby test the information-processing
account of “unaware” autonomic conditioning during extinction, a dichotic listening task was implemented, with the CSs presented
in the unattended channel (ear), while the subject had to perform a semantic differential reaction task in an attended-to
channel (other ear). In early extinction, the electrodermal response occurring at an interval of 9–15 s after CS onset (i.e.,
following placement of the US during acquisition) and the finger-pulse-volume response occurring at an interval of 4–11 s
after CS onset both showed reliable conditioning, but reaction-time and subjective-report data for the recognized critical
words indicated serial rather than parallel processing of the CSs during extinction. 相似文献
78.
Three robot studies on visual prediction are presented. In all of them, a visual forward model is used, which predicts the visual consequences of saccade-like camera movements. This forward model works by remapping visual information between the pre- and postsaccadic retinal images; at an abstract modeling level, this process is closely related to neurons whose visual receptive fields shift in anticipation of saccades. In the robot studies, predictive remapping is used (1) in the context of saccade adaptation, to reidentify target objects after saccades are carried out; (2) for a model of grasping, in which both fixated and non-fixated target objects are processed by the same foveal mechanism; and (3) in a computational architecture for mental imagery, which generates “gripper appearances” internally without real sensory inflow. The robotic experiments and their underlying computational models are discussed with regard to predictive remapping in the brain, transsaccadic memory, and attention. The results confirm that visual prediction is a mechanism that has to be considered in the design of artificial cognitive agents and the modeling of information processing in the human visual system. 相似文献
79.
In this field-study, we analysed the interrelations between gender role self-concept, gender-role conflict, and well-being in male primary school teachers. Our sample comprised N = 81 respondents from an urban area in Germany (average age: 42.9 years; average tenure: 11.4 years). In Germany, primary school teaching is a numerically female-dominated occupation. We found that masculinity, femininity, and androgyny positively influenced well-being, while gender-role conflict was negatively interrelated with well-being. We expected that gender-role conflict weakens the positive effects of masculinity and femininity. Unexpectedly, we even found a detrimental effect of femininity when gender-role conflict was high: For respondents reporting comparatively high gender-role conflict, femininity was associated with lower work satisfaction. 相似文献
80.
This study investigated grandiose narcissism from a categorical perspective. We tested whether subgroups of narcissists can be distinguished that differ in their expressions of more agentic (narcissistic admiration, ADM) and more antagonistic (narcissistic rivalry, RIV) pathways of narcissism. We analysed three German samples (total N = 2211; Mage = 26; 70% female) and one US sample (N = 971; Mage = 35; 74% female) using latent class analysis. Four subgroups of narcissists were consistently identified across samples from Germany and the United States: low narcissists, moderate narcissists primarily characterized by agentic aspects (ADM), moderate narcissists characterized by both agentic and antagonistic aspects (ADM + RIV), and high narcissists. The subgroups were systematically related to a number of personality traits (e.g. Machiavellianism, impulsivity) and adjustment indicators (e.g. self‐esteem, empathy). Members in the moderate narcissists—ADM subgroup showed the most adaptive characteristics while members in the moderate narcissists—ADM + RIV subgroup showed the most maladaptive characteristics. Investigating grandiose narcissism—a primarily quantitative trait—from a categorical perspective can yield valuable insights that would otherwise be overlooked. In addition, our results underline the utility of a self‐regulatory process approach to grandiose narcissism that distinguishes between agentic and antagonistic dynamics. Copyright © 2016 European Association of Personality Psychology 相似文献