全文获取类型
收费全文 | 412篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
420篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 41篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 19篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 9篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1964年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有420条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
341.
Werner Nell 《Journal of Psychology in Africa》2014,24(1):82-91
This study explored the sources of meaning in life among a group of 243 South African university students, using a sequential exploratory mixed methods design. First, data from semi-structured questionnaires (n=40) were subjected to qualitative content analysis; next, from the emerging themes, a quantitative questionnaire was developed and administered to 203 students. Mean scores were computed and compared across gender and cultural groups. Relationships, especially with family, as well as hope, education, achievement and religion were found to be most important sources of meaning, followed by service, creative self-expression, material possessions, hobbies, health and pets. The qualitative analysis revealed that most of these sources were valued more for their perceived instrumental utility than for their intrinsic qualities. 相似文献
342.
Werner Kierski 《Counselling and Psychotherapy Research》2014,14(2):111-118
Aims: There is currently a lack of clarity surrounding how men experience anxiety. This is caused by epistemological limitations with the perspectives employed to explore male anxiety, but equally by a lack of empirical research and uncertainty of how to define masculinity. In order to explain male anxiety more effectively this study needed to overcome these limitations by both incorporating the notion that there are multiple masculinities, through which men experience specific anxieties, and allowing men to talk directly about their experiences. This study focused on one kind of masculine identity: that of male psychotherapists. Research design: A hermeneutic phenomenological method was employed. Eight male psychotherapists of various modalities were interviewed who work in a number of therapeutic contexts. Results: Anxiety patterns are complex and comprise of feelings that are very difficult to deal with. Yet, anxiety is also an impetus for learning and improvement. Conclusions: The few existing ideas of male anxiety fail to capture the complexity of how men experience anxiety. Concepts of male anxiety need to be defined with more accuracy. 相似文献
343.
344.
Annekathrin Schacht Olaf Dimigen Werner Sommer 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2010,10(3):349-356
It has been suggested that cognitive conflicts require effortful processing and, therefore, are aversive (Botvinick, 2007).
In the present study, we compared conflicts emerging from the inhibition of a predominant response tendency in a go/no-go
task with those between incompatible response activations in a Simon task in a within-subjects design, using the same type
of stimuli. Whereas no-go trials elicited reduced skin conductance and pupillometric responses, but prolonged corrugator muscle
activity, as compared with go trials, incompatible and compatible Simon trials were indistinguishable with respect to these
parameters. Furthermore, the conflictsensitive N2 components of the event-related brain potential were similar in amplitude,
but showed significantly different scalp distributions, indicating dissociable neural generator systems. The present findings
suggest the involvement of different emotional and cognitive processes in both types of cognitive conflicts—none being aversive,
however. In addition, the N2 findings call into question claims of common monitoring systems for all kinds of cognitive conflicts. 相似文献
345.
346.
347.
348.
Ohne Zusammenfassung 相似文献
349.
350.
Benedikt Werner Elisabeth von Ramin Adriaan Spruyt Klaus Rothermund 《Cognition & emotion》2018,32(1):222-230
After 30 years of research, the mechanisms underlying the evaluative priming effect are still a topic of debate. In this study, we tested whether the evaluative priming effect can result from (uncontrolled) associative relatedness rather than evaluative congruency. Stimuli that share the same evaluative connotation are more likely to show some degree of non-evaluative associative relatedness than stimuli that have a different evaluative connotation. Therefore, unless associative relatedness is explicitly controlled for, evaluative priming effects reported in earlier research may be driven by associative relatedness instead of evaluative relatedness. To address this possibility, we performed an evaluative priming study in which evaluative congruency and associative relatedness were manipulated independently from each other. The valent/neutral categorisation task was used to ensure evaluative stimulus processing in the absence of response priming effects. Results showed an effect of associative relatedness but no (overall) effect of evaluative congruency. Our findings highlight the importance of controlling for associative relatedness when testing for evaluative priming effects. 相似文献