首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1436篇
  免费   48篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   10篇
  2021年   16篇
  2020年   17篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   36篇
  2017年   51篇
  2016年   41篇
  2015年   32篇
  2014年   40篇
  2013年   132篇
  2012年   75篇
  2011年   62篇
  2010年   41篇
  2009年   52篇
  2008年   65篇
  2007年   70篇
  2006年   49篇
  2005年   49篇
  2004年   54篇
  2003年   43篇
  2002年   53篇
  2001年   31篇
  2000年   33篇
  1999年   24篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   23篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   18篇
  1994年   18篇
  1993年   12篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   15篇
  1990年   16篇
  1989年   15篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   14篇
  1986年   13篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   16篇
  1982年   9篇
  1981年   11篇
  1980年   6篇
  1979年   6篇
  1978年   8篇
  1976年   6篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   6篇
  1972年   5篇
排序方式: 共有1484条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
841.
842.
843.
844.
  • This is a study of a small group of self‐identified butch lesbians in their consumption of tattoos. The emotion‐laden experience of negotiating social symbolism and self‐identity (Elliott, 1997 ) in the context of hyperrealism (Baudrillard, 1983 ) has given rise to what Frankl ( 1984 ) has described as “existential vacuums.” Frankl suggested that these “existential vacuums” can be filled with a higher level of meaning. An artifact of consumption, the tattoo, is used by the informants in this study as a component of bricolage in the DIY process of constructing a new self (Elliott, 1997 ), with the entire process of being tattooed – particularly the hyper‐stimulation and pain of the procedure – further “existentializing” a new existence of “imagined masculinity.” This new existence that transcends suffering and assigns new meaning to life is the principle idea of Frankl's logotherapy (Frankl, 1984 ; Barnes, 2000 ; Blair, 2004 ).
Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
845.
On s’est demandé, dans cette recherche, si un séminaire exploitant les erreurs pouvait améliorer les performances. 59 pompiers expérimentés en formation sur les problèmes de commandement ont participéà cette étude. Des scènes de guerre ont été conçues à partir d’évènements réels pour illustrer des décisions hiérarchiques dont les suites furent heureuses ou malheureuses. Deux méthodes de formation ont été comparées et évaluées. Un premier groupe a été confrontéà des études de cas contenant des erreurs de management aux conséquences graves sur l’intervention contre l’incendie (formation avec erreurs) alors qu’un second groupe était soumis à la même série d’études de cas, sinon que les cas décrivaient des situations gérées correctement avec leurs conséquences (formation sans erreur). Les résultats confortent l’hypothèse qu’il vaut mieux apprendre à partir des erreurs des autres plutôt qu’à partir de leurs réussites. On analyse les leçons à en tirer pour la formation. The study examines whether error exposure training can enhance adaptive performance. Fifty‐nine experienced fire‐fighters undergoing training for incident command participated in the study. War stories were developed based on real events to illustrate successful and unsuccessful incident command decisions. Two training methodologies were compared and evaluated. One group was trained using case studies that depicted incidents containing errors of management with severe consequences in fire‐fighting outcomes (error‐story training) while a second group was exposed to the same set of case studies except that the case studies depicted the incidents being managed without errors and their consequences (errorless‐story training). The results provide some support for the hypothesis that it is better to learn from other people's errors than from their successes. Implications for training are discussed.  相似文献   
846.
This study was designed to determine if movement planning strategies incorporating the use of visual feedback during manual aiming are specific to individual movements. Advance information about target location and visual context was manipulated using precues. Participants exhibited a shorter reaction time and a longer movement time when they were certain of the target location and that vision would be available. The longer movement time was associated with greater time after peak velocity. Under conditions of uncertainty, participants prepared for the worst-case scenario. That is, they spent more time organizing their movements and produced trajectories that would be expected from greater open-loop control. Our results are consistent with hierarchical movement planning in which knowledge of the movement goal is an essential ingredient of visual feedback utilization.  相似文献   
847.
Standard theory of planned behavior (TPB) questions to elicit salient behavioral beliefs may elicit instrumental consequences of behavior, and overlook affective consequences. Two hundred thirteen English adults (35 to 75 years of age) completed a questionnaire that contained closed measures of TPB constructs, and open‐ended questions that asked not only about advantages and disadvantages, but also what respondents would like or enjoy and dislike or hate about being more physically active. Beliefs elicited by affective questions were associated more strongly with a closed affective attitude scale. Beliefs elicited by instrumental questions were associated more strongly with a closed instrumental attitude scale. Closed measures of the standard TPB variables explained 48% of the variance in intention to increase physical activity, while affective attitude explained an additional 11% of the variance. Applications of the TPB should consider affective and not just instrumental determinants of behavior.  相似文献   
848.
849.
Short-term memory for the timing of irregular sequences of signals has been said to be more accurate when the signals are auditory than when they are visual. No support for this contention was obtained when the signals were beeps versus flashes (Experiments 1 and 3) nor when they were sets of spoken versus typewritten digits (Experiments 4 and 5). On the other hand, support was obtained both for beeps versus flashes (Experiments 2 and 5) and for repetitions of a single spoken digit versus repetitions of a single typewritten digit (Experiment 6) when the subjects silently mouthed a nominally irrelevant item during sequence presentation. Also, the timing of sequences of auditory signals, whether verbal (Experiment 7) or nonverbal (Experiments 8 and 9), was more accurately remembered when the signals within each sequence were identical. The findings are considered from a functional perspective.  相似文献   
850.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号