首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   306篇
  免费   6篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   8篇
  2018年   2篇
  2017年   10篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   33篇
  2012年   13篇
  2011年   5篇
  2010年   8篇
  2009年   11篇
  2008年   12篇
  2007年   15篇
  2006年   7篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   10篇
  2003年   8篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   12篇
  1999年   6篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   3篇
  1994年   5篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   2篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   3篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   5篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   5篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   2篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   3篇
  1969年   3篇
  1967年   2篇
  1964年   2篇
排序方式: 共有313条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
121.
Individual differences in desire for feedback did not exhibit any significant correlations with a variety of self-report personality dimensions. Additional survey studies indicated that a higher proportion of individuals in nonclinical as compared to clinical samples reported a strong desire to receive feedback about themselves. Subsequent studies revealed that the higher as compared to lower desire for feedback individuals: (1) were more willing to seek psychological help and to participate in therapy once in treatment; (2) exhibited more positive expectancies for change at the beginning of therapy; and (3) were more “responsive” to diagnostic feedback. Within the limitations of the present findings, the potential “adaptiveness” of desire for feedback is discussed and the negative motivational therapeutic consequences associated with a low desire for feedback are explored.  相似文献   
122.
123.
Giving eyewitnesses confirming feedback after they make a lineup identification (e.g., "Good. You identified the actual suspect.") inflates not only their recollections of how confident they were at the time of the identification, but also other testimony-relevant judgments, such as how good their view was, how much attention they paid during witnessing, and how quickly they identified the suspect. We replicated this postidentification-feedback effect with eyewitnesses who had made false identifications (N = 156), adding critical conditions in which after the identification but prior to the feedback, some eyewitnesses were given instructions to privately think about their confidence, their view, and other matters. Other eyewitnesses were given the same thought instructions subsequent to the feedback manipulation. Prior thought served to mitigate the effects of feedback, but subsequent thought did not. In addition, even without feedback, privately thinking about confidence had some confidence-inflating properties of its own.  相似文献   
124.
Although worry is the central feature of Generalised Anxiety Disorder (GAD), little is known about the factors that contribute to pathological or problematic worry. In a recent cognitive model of GAD, Wells, A. (1995) proposed that negative appraisal of worrying itself (meta-worry or type 2 worry) should be distinguished from other types of worrying (type 1 worry). A central feature of this model is the idea that individuals with GAD hold rigid positive beliefs about the usefulness of worrying as a coping strategy. However, these individuals also hold negative beliefs and appraise worrying as uncontrollable and dangerous. This combination of cognitions and associated responses leads to an increased frequency and generality of worrying, and thus to the pathological worry characteristic of GAD. This paper reports a preliminary test of the hypothesis that meta-worry contributes to problematic and pathological worrying, and this relationship is independent of the frequency of other types of worry. In testing for associations between worry dimensions we controlled for overlaps with Trait anxiety, and the controllability of worrying. Results of a series of regression analyses support the hypothesis that pathological worry is associated with meta-worry and this association is independent of Trait-anxiety and type 1 worry. The clinical implications of these data are briefly discussed.  相似文献   
125.
This study tested the hypothesis that how a discussion of a marital conflict begins--in its first few minutes--is a predictor of divorce. The marital conflict discussion of 124 newlywed couples was coded using the Specific Affect Coding System, and the data were divided into positive, negative, and positive-minus-negative affect totals for five 3-minute intervals. It was possible to predict marital outcome over a 6-year period using just the first 3 minutes of data for both husbands and wives. For husbands this prediction improved as the groups diverged in the remaining 12 minutes; for wives the prediction remained equally powerful for the remaining 12 minutes as it had been in the first 3 minutes.  相似文献   
126.
127.
Viewed from a behavioral perspective alcoholism is a behavioral disorder which should be influenced by the same range of environmental and historical variables which affect other operant behaviors. Therefore, environmental stimulus circumstances should play a significant role in influencing alcoholics' disposition to drink. In particular, stimuli which have previously been associated with drinking behavior should come to exert some controlling influence over subsequent disposition to drink. Relapse to substance abuse is often believed to be precipitated by exposure to stimulus circumstances previously associated with the drug or its use. This hypothesis has received its greatest emphasis in the area of narcotics abuse (Wikler, 1965). but presumably it is relevant also to the case of alcohol abuse. Research concerning abusers' reactions to alcohol-related or drinking-related stimuli has been minimal. Ludwig et al. (1974) have presented data suggesting that alcohol-related stimuli interact with priming doses of ethanol to increase alcoholics' disposition to drink. Miller et al. (1974), however, failed to observe a significant effect of drinking-related stimuli upon alcoholics' disposition to drink.

In the field of behavior therapy, relaxation and stimulus-exposure techniques (e.g. systematic desensitization) are often recommended as techniques for reducing the effect of environmental stimuli upon an individual's behavior. However, data are lacking concerning the effect of relaxation training upon alcoholics. The present study uses psychophysiological procedures to investigate three issues concerning the effect of drinking-related stimuli and relaxation instructions upon alcoholics: (1) the effect of a single session of systematic relaxation instructions upon the electromyographic tension levels of abstinent alcoholics; (2) the effect of drinking-related auditory stimuli upon the electromyographic tension levels of abstinent alcoholics; and (3) the influence of prior relaxation instructions upon the EMG responses of abstinent alcoholics exposed to drinking-related stimuli.  相似文献   

128.
Hypotheses advanced concerning the functions and advantages of the two-joint (and multi-joint) muscles in the lower limb include transferring energy, ease of control, muscle bulk reduction and decreased velocity of contraction. The aim of this investigation was to assess quantitatively the generality of one such suggestion seen in the literature. It was hypothesized that two-joint muscles would be recruited preferentially when they produced appropriate moments at the joints they crossed. This organizing strategy was used to partition the sagittal plane joint moment at the hip, knee and ankle between the one- and two-joint muscles crossing those joints. If the conditions of the strategy were not met, the moment was considered to be producted by one-joint muscles only. Ten representative muscles were modelled: tibialis anterior, soleus, gastrocnemius, short head of biceps femoris, vasti, rectus femoris, long head of biceps femoris, sartorius, gluteus maximus and iliopsoas. A number of static loading and walking conditions were recorded and then compared to simultaneously measured linear envelope EMG records of each activity. The joint moments were determined from a sagittal plane kinetic analysis using cinematography and measurements of the ground reaction force. Overall, the strategy partitioned the moment between the one- and two-joint muscles in accordance with the EMG records. The strategy tended to underestimate the contributions of the one-joint musculature, implying the existence of other important control strategies, such as cocontraction of antagonists for joint stability, or of synergistic activation to share the joint moment. It was, however, observed that predicted activity of two-joint musculature did agree well with recorded EMG activity.  相似文献   
129.
130.
A number of basic engagement techniques in family therapy are identified and illustrated. These include (1) techniques that enable the therapist to achieve a position of influence with family members or to gain entry into the family system, (2) techniques utilized in teaching the family how to behave in the therapy setting, and (3) the tactics the therapist may use to manage difficult moments in the session. Issues in the relationship between technique and the theory and practice of family intervention are also examined.A videotape (Wells, R.A., & Page, M.H.Engaging the Family in the Helping Process: Basic Techniques. Pittsburgh, PA: UIRC, 1979) is available from the author. Discussion with such colleagues as Patrick McNamee, Larry Pacoe, Mary Page, and Susan Schilling has been invaluable.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号