全文获取类型
收费全文 | 940篇 |
免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
954篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 12篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 21篇 |
2015年 | 17篇 |
2014年 | 14篇 |
2013年 | 82篇 |
2012年 | 24篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 17篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 31篇 |
2007年 | 41篇 |
2006年 | 22篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 34篇 |
2002年 | 24篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 18篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
1957年 | 7篇 |
1956年 | 8篇 |
1955年 | 7篇 |
1954年 | 7篇 |
1953年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有954条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
931.
932.
Wayne Wu 《No?s (Detroit, Mich.)》2011,45(1):50-76
I argue that when perception plays a guiding role in intentional bodily action, it is a necessary part of that action. The argument begins with a challenge that necessarily arises for embodied agents, what I call the Many‐Many Problem. The Problem is named after its most common case where agents face too many perceptual inputs and too many possible behavioral outputs. Action requires a solution to the Many‐Many Problem by selection of a specific linkage between input and output. In bodily action the agent perceptually selects, and in this way perceptually attends to, relevant information so as to guide the execution of specific movements. Since perceptual attention is a necessary part of solving the Many‐Many Problem, it is a necessary part of bodily action. Indeed, the process of implementing a solution to the Many‐Many Problem, as constrained by the agent's motivational state, just is the agent's performing an intentional bodily action in the relevant way. 相似文献
933.
Harris WG Jones JW Klion R Arnold DW Camara W Cunningham MR 《The Journal of applied psychology》2012,97(3):531-536
Van Iddekinge, Roth, Raymark, and Odle-Dusseau's (2012) meta-analysis of pre-employment integrity test results confirmed that such tests are meaningfully related to counterproductive work behavior. The article also offered some cautionary conclusions, which appear to stem from the limited scope of the authors' focus and the specific research procedures used. Issues discussed in this commentary include the following: (a) test publishers' provision of studies for meta-analytic consideration; (b) errors and questions in the coding of statistics from past studies; (c) debatable corrections for unreliable criterion measures; (d) exclusion of laboratory, contrasted-groups, unit-level, and time-series studies of counterproductive behavior; (e) under-emphasis on the prediction of counterproductive workplace behaviors compared with job performance, training outcomes, and turnover; (f) overlooking the industry practice of deploying integrity scales with other valid predictors of employee outcomes; (g) implication that integrity test publishers produce biased research results; (h) incomplete presentation of integrity tests' resistance to faking; and (i) omission of data indicating applicants' favorable response to integrity tests, the tests' lack of adverse impact, and the positive business impact of integrity testing. This commentary, therefore, offers an alternate perspective, addresses omissions and apparent inaccuracies, and urges a return to the use of diverse methodologies to evaluate the validity of integrity tests and other psychometric instruments. 相似文献
934.
Wayne P. Nadler 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2012,19(2):381-383
Comments are offered to clarify the learning model proposed by Arch and Craske (2011) based on extensive clinical experience with the CBT model for treating panic disorder developed by Barlow and Craske (1990). Suggestions are made regarding treatment targets and several cases are offered as examples of how choice of treatment target can make a significant difference when panic appears refractory. 相似文献
935.
Although mood disorders constitute leading causes of disability, until recently little was known about their pathogenesis. The delineation of anatomical networks that support emotional behavior (mainly derived from animal studies) and the development of neuroimaging technologies that allow in vivo characterization of anatomy, physiology, and neurochemistry in human subjects with mood disorders have enabled significant advances towards elucidating the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder (MDD) and bipolar disorder (BD). In this review, we integrate insights from human and animal studies, which collectively suggest that MDD and BD involve dysfunction within an extended network including the medial prefrontal cortex and anatomically-related limbic, striatal, thalamic and basal forebrain structures. 相似文献
936.
937.
938.
James O Prochaska Wayne F Velicer Joseph S Rossi Colleen A Redding Geoffrey W Greene Susan R Rossi Xiaowu Sun Joseph L Fava Robert Laforge Brett A Plummer 《Health psychology》2004,23(5):503-516
Three stage-based expert system interventions for smoking, high-fat diet, and unsafe sun exposure were evaluated in a sample of 2,460 parents of teenagers. Eighty-four percent of the eligible parents were enrolled in a 2-arm randomized control trial, with the treatment group receiving individualized feedback reports for each of their relevant behaviors at 0, 6, and 12 months as well as a multiple behavior manual. At 24 months, the expert system outperformed the comparison condition across all 3 risk behaviors, resulting in 22% of the participants in action or maintenance for smoking (vs. 16% for the comparison condition), 34% for diet (vs. 26%), and 30% for sun exposure (vs. 22%). Proactive, home-based, and stage-matched expert systems can produce significant multiple behavior changes in at-risk populations where the majority of participants are not prepared to change. 相似文献
939.
Wayne G. Rollins 《Pastoral Psychology》2003,51(6):455-468
Building on the theoretical basis spelled out in my first article on Hal Childs' The Myth of the Historical Jesus and the Evolution of Consciousness, the present article engages in dialogue with Crossan's 2000 autobiography, A Long Way From Tipperary. The dialogue focuses on six proposals emerging from Childs' work that advocate the inclusion of psychological realism in rethinking the task, practice, and outcome of historical Jesus research. The six proposals are as follows: first, that psychological realism is an essential part of historical realism; second, that unconscious factors are to be considered at work in the viewer as well as in the viewed in historical Jesus research; third, that every reconstruction of the historical Jesus is mythic; fourth, that the preunderstanding that the Jesus scholar brings to historical Jesus research is generated within a hermeneutical circle constituted by a vast web of relationships, purposes, and meanings that include every aspect of the scholar's life; fifth, that the final goal of historical Jesus research is not the facts about the historic Jesus, but the meaning of these facts as archetypal images for self-understanding, world-understanding, and the evolution of consciousness; and sixth, that the purpose of the Gospel is to evoke new archetypal projections in the reader that can lead to new incarnations of the archetypal Self awakened and informed by the story of Jesus, often as recovered by the Jesus historian. 相似文献
940.
Stephen N. Elliott Wayne C. Piersel Gloria A. Galvin 《Journal of School Psychology》1983,21(2):99-105
The practice of psychologically re-evaluating students receiving special education services has been mandated by P.L. 94–142 and further operationalized by state departments of education. Intuitively, such a requirement appears sound, for psychologists occasionally make mistakes, children change over time, and efficacy of special education services is variable. Yet given the other mandated practices, such as yearly IEP updates, and the fact that many school psychologists spend a majority of their time testing, we wondered how important three-year psychological re-evaluations are considered by school psychologists. In the present study 40 psychologists across four states were surveyed concerning their perceptions and practices regarding re-evaluations. The psychologists' responses to a 16-item questionnaire indicated that re-evaluations comprised a substantial portion of their evaluations, involved a considerable amount of time, and were perceived as an important check on a child's diagnosis and placement. Re-evaluations and initial evaluations were described as involving nearly identical tests and related activities. 相似文献