全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1576篇 |
免费 | 89篇 |
专业分类
1665篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 32篇 |
2019年 | 46篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 84篇 |
2016年 | 51篇 |
2015年 | 42篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 178篇 |
2012年 | 82篇 |
2011年 | 93篇 |
2010年 | 42篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 62篇 |
2006年 | 53篇 |
2005年 | 75篇 |
2004年 | 57篇 |
2003年 | 53篇 |
2002年 | 43篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1665条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
Children sometimes have trouble switching from one task to another, despite demonstrating an awareness of current task demands.
This behavior could reflect problems either directly inhibiting previously relevant information or sufficiently activating
graded working memory representations for the current task. We tested competing predictions from each account, using a computerized
card-sorting task in which we assessed children’s task switching abilities and their response speed to simple questions about
current task demands. All children answered these questions correctly, but children who successfully switched tasks responded
more quickly to questions than did children who perseverated on previous tasks, even after factoring out processing speed
and age. This reaction time difference supports graded working memory accounts, with stronger representations of current task
demands aiding both task-switching and responses to questions. This result poses a challenge for directed inhibition accounts,
because nothing needs to be inhibited to answer simple questions that lack conflicting information. 相似文献
93.
Vitevitch and Luce (1998) showed that the probability with which phonemes co-occur in the language (phonotactic probability)
affects the speed with which words and nonwords are named. Words with high phonotactic probabilities between phonemes were
named more slowly than words with low probabilities, whereas with nonwords, just the opposite was found. To reproduce this
reversal in performance, a model would seem to require not merely sublexical representations, but sublexical representations
that are relatively independent of lexical representations. ARTphone (Grossberg, Boardman, & Cohen, 1997) is designed to meet
these requirements. In this study, we used a technique called parameter space partitioning to analyze ARTphone’s behavior
and to learn if it can mimic human behavior and, if so, to understand how. To perform best, differences in sublexical node
probabilities must be amplified relative to lexical node probabilities to offset the additional source of inhibition (from
top-down masking) that is found at the sublexical level. 相似文献
94.
Kristin R. Egemo‐Helm Raymond G. Miltenberger Peter Knudson Nicholas Finstrom Candice Jostad Brigitte Johnson 《Behavioral Interventions》2007,22(2):99-119
The current study evaluated a behavioral skills training (BST) program in combination with in situ training to teach sexual abuse prevention skills to five women with mild to moderate mental retardation. In situ assessments were conducted following BST and in situ training sessions were conducted for those who were unable to demonstrate the skills in the natural setting. The results showed that generalization of the safety skills to the natural setting occurred for three of the five participants following one to two in situ training sessions. One participant required 12 in situ training sessions and three booster training sessions to reach criterion level. Three of four participants assessed 1 month following training maintained the skills. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
95.
Nicholas Rescher 《Argumentation》1987,1(3):303-316
Consistency is often pictured as an indispensable requisite for rationality. The paper argues that this is overly rigoristic. Inconsistency can be treated as a matter of isolable singularities rather than an all-destructive disaster. The paper, supports and illustrates a perspective on which consistency can be seen as a desideratum rather than a totaly non-negotiable demand. The argumentation of the paper casts consistency in the role of a cognitive ideal rather than a sine qua non condition of rational process. 相似文献
96.
Nicholas Long Rex Forehand Robert Fauber Gene H. Brody 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):15-27
The self-perceived and independently observed cognitive and social competence of young adolescents as a function of parental conflict and recent divorce was investigated. Subjects were 40 young adolescents between the ages of 11 years 1 month and 15 years 1 month. A 2×2 factorial design was used, with the independent variables being parental marital status (married vs. recently divorced) and parental conflict (high vs. low). Dependent variables included the following measures of adolescent competence: adolescent-completed measures of self-perceived competence, teacher-completed measures, behavioral observations, and school grades. The results indicated that the level of parental conflict, rather than parental marital status, appears to be the critical variable associated with adolescents' independently observed levels of cognitive and social competence. In regard to adolescents' self-perceived levels of cognitive and social competence, parental marital status was found to be the critical variable. The implications of these findings are discussed.This research was supported by the William T. Grant Foundation and the University of Georgia's Institute for Behavioral Research. 相似文献
97.
98.
Michelle Luciano Margaret J. Wright Gina M. Geffen Laurie B. Geffen Glen A. Smith David M. Evans Nicholas G. Martin 《Intelligence》2003,31(6):589-605
The phenotypic and genetic factor structure of performance on five Multidimensional Aptitude Battery (MAB) subtests and one Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R) subtest was explored in 390 adolescent twin pairs (184 monozygotic [MZ]; 206 dizygotic (DZ)). The temporal stability of these measures was derived from a subsample of 49 twin pairs, with test–retest correlations ranging from .67 to .85. A phenotypic factor model, in which performance and verbal factors were correlated, provided a good fit to the data. Genetic modeling was based on the phenotypic factor structure, but also took into account the additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and unique environmental (E) parameters derived from a fully saturated ACE model. The best fitting model was characterized by a genetic correlated two-factor structure with specific effects, a general common environmental factor, and overlapping unique environmental effects. Results are compared to multivariate genetic models reported in children and adults, with the most notable difference being the growing importance of common genes influencing diverse abilities in adolescence. 相似文献
99.
On-line syntactic processing in aphasia: studies with auditory moving window presentation 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Twenty-eight aphasic patients with left hemisphere strokes and matched control subjects were tested on an auditory moving windows task in which successive phrases of a sentence were presented in response to subjects' self-paced button presses and subjects made timed judgments regarding the plausibility of each sentence. Pairs of sentences were presented that differed in syntactic complexity. Patients made more errors and/or took longer in making the plausibility judgments than controls, and were more affected than controls by the syntactic complexity of a sentence in these judgments. Normal subjects showed effects of syntactic structure in self-paced listening. On-line syntactic effects differed in patients as a function of their comprehension level. High-performing patients showed the same effects as normal control subjects; low performing patients did not show the same effects of syntactic structure. On-line syntactic effects also differed in patients as a function of their clinical diagnosis. Broca's aphasic patients' on-line performances suggested that they were not processing complex syntactic structures on-line, while fluent aphasics' performances suggested that their comprehension impairment occurred after on-line processing was accomplished. The results indicate that many aphasic patients retain their ability to process syntactic structure on-line, and that different groups of patients with syntactic comprehension disorders show different patterns of on-line syntactic processing. 相似文献
100.
Participants switched frequently between high/low and odd/even classification of a digit. The interval between a task cue and the next digit varied between blocks. In Experiment 1, the task switched predictably every two, four, or eight trials. In Experiment 2, switching predictably every four trials was compared with random switching. With predictable switching, the cost was limited to the first trial of a run. Random switching produced a more gradual approach to asymptotic performance. After one performance, control processes attenuate the resulting change in task-set bias if a further switch is likely, but this strategic modulation is soon overwhelmed by task-set priming through further performances. Preparation reduced switch costs but not interference from the irrelevant attribute: Control of interference appears to be reactive, not proactive. Switch costs did not increase with run length, suggesting that retrieval of the task set last associated with the stimulus did not contribute to switch costs. 相似文献