全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1641篇 |
免费 | 46篇 |
专业分类
1687篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 23篇 |
2021年 | 18篇 |
2020年 | 32篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 84篇 |
2016年 | 52篇 |
2015年 | 42篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 181篇 |
2012年 | 82篇 |
2011年 | 93篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 62篇 |
2008年 | 72篇 |
2007年 | 62篇 |
2006年 | 54篇 |
2005年 | 75篇 |
2004年 | 57篇 |
2003年 | 53篇 |
2002年 | 43篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1687条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Anthony J. Goreczny Phillip J. Brantley Ray R. Buss William F. Waters 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1988,10(3):259-267
Although stress and anxiety have long been assumed to play an exacerbatory role in asthma, no study has systematically documented that daily exacerbations of asthma symptoms are related to stress and/or anxiety. In this study, 24 airways obstruction patients (12 asthmatics and 12 chronic obstructive pulmonary disease patients) were instructed to monitor the severity of daily respiratory symptoms. In addition, subjects recorded their daily anxiety level and the number and perceived impact of daily stressors. The results showed that although there were differences between high- and low-stress days for both groups, there were no differences between groups on symptom severity or between high- and low-anxiety days, as measured by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Thus, although the number and impact of daily stressors were found to be directly associated with the severity of asthma symptoms, anxiety does not appear to have a direct role in the exacerbation of asthma. The findings failed to support the anxiety theory of asthma but provided an explanation for the poor results obtained in previous treatment studies which employed anxiety management with asthmatics. 相似文献
73.
74.
Children sometimes have trouble switching from one task to another, despite demonstrating an awareness of current task demands.
This behavior could reflect problems either directly inhibiting previously relevant information or sufficiently activating
graded working memory representations for the current task. We tested competing predictions from each account, using a computerized
card-sorting task in which we assessed children’s task switching abilities and their response speed to simple questions about
current task demands. All children answered these questions correctly, but children who successfully switched tasks responded
more quickly to questions than did children who perseverated on previous tasks, even after factoring out processing speed
and age. This reaction time difference supports graded working memory accounts, with stronger representations of current task
demands aiding both task-switching and responses to questions. This result poses a challenge for directed inhibition accounts,
because nothing needs to be inhibited to answer simple questions that lack conflicting information. 相似文献
75.
Vitevitch and Luce (1998) showed that the probability with which phonemes co-occur in the language (phonotactic probability)
affects the speed with which words and nonwords are named. Words with high phonotactic probabilities between phonemes were
named more slowly than words with low probabilities, whereas with nonwords, just the opposite was found. To reproduce this
reversal in performance, a model would seem to require not merely sublexical representations, but sublexical representations
that are relatively independent of lexical representations. ARTphone (Grossberg, Boardman, & Cohen, 1997) is designed to meet
these requirements. In this study, we used a technique called parameter space partitioning to analyze ARTphone’s behavior
and to learn if it can mimic human behavior and, if so, to understand how. To perform best, differences in sublexical node
probabilities must be amplified relative to lexical node probabilities to offset the additional source of inhibition (from
top-down masking) that is found at the sublexical level. 相似文献
76.
Nicholas Long Rex Forehand Robert Fauber Gene H. Brody 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):15-27
The self-perceived and independently observed cognitive and social competence of young adolescents as a function of parental conflict and recent divorce was investigated. Subjects were 40 young adolescents between the ages of 11 years 1 month and 15 years 1 month. A 2×2 factorial design was used, with the independent variables being parental marital status (married vs. recently divorced) and parental conflict (high vs. low). Dependent variables included the following measures of adolescent competence: adolescent-completed measures of self-perceived competence, teacher-completed measures, behavioral observations, and school grades. The results indicated that the level of parental conflict, rather than parental marital status, appears to be the critical variable associated with adolescents' independently observed levels of cognitive and social competence. In regard to adolescents' self-perceived levels of cognitive and social competence, parental marital status was found to be the critical variable. The implications of these findings are discussed.This research was supported by the William T. Grant Foundation and the University of Georgia's Institute for Behavioral Research. 相似文献
77.
Michelle Luciano Margaret J. Wright Gina M. Geffen Laurie B. Geffen Glen A. Smith David M. Evans Nicholas G. Martin 《Intelligence》2003,31(6):589-605
The phenotypic and genetic factor structure of performance on five Multidimensional Aptitude Battery (MAB) subtests and one Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised (WAIS-R) subtest was explored in 390 adolescent twin pairs (184 monozygotic [MZ]; 206 dizygotic (DZ)). The temporal stability of these measures was derived from a subsample of 49 twin pairs, with test–retest correlations ranging from .67 to .85. A phenotypic factor model, in which performance and verbal factors were correlated, provided a good fit to the data. Genetic modeling was based on the phenotypic factor structure, but also took into account the additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and unique environmental (E) parameters derived from a fully saturated ACE model. The best fitting model was characterized by a genetic correlated two-factor structure with specific effects, a general common environmental factor, and overlapping unique environmental effects. Results are compared to multivariate genetic models reported in children and adults, with the most notable difference being the growing importance of common genes influencing diverse abilities in adolescence. 相似文献
78.
Participants switched frequently between high/low and odd/even classification of a digit. The interval between a task cue and the next digit varied between blocks. In Experiment 1, the task switched predictably every two, four, or eight trials. In Experiment 2, switching predictably every four trials was compared with random switching. With predictable switching, the cost was limited to the first trial of a run. Random switching produced a more gradual approach to asymptotic performance. After one performance, control processes attenuate the resulting change in task-set bias if a further switch is likely, but this strategic modulation is soon overwhelmed by task-set priming through further performances. Preparation reduced switch costs but not interference from the irrelevant attribute: Control of interference appears to be reactive, not proactive. Switch costs did not increase with run length, suggesting that retrieval of the task set last associated with the stimulus did not contribute to switch costs. 相似文献
79.
Dodge, in 1916, suggested that the French term 'saccade' should be used for describing the rapid movements of the eyes that occur while reading. Previously he had referred to these as type I movements. Javal had used the term 'saccade' in 1879, when describing experiments conducted in his laboratory by Lamare. Accordingly, Javal has been rightly credited with assigning the term to rapid eye movements. In English these rapid rotations had been called jerks, and they had been observed and measured before Lamare's studies of reading. Rapid sweeps of the eyes occur as one phase of nystagmus; they were observed by Wells in 1792 who used an afterimage technique, and they were illustrated by Crum Brown in 1878. Afterimages were used in nineteenth-century research on eye movements and eye position; they were also employed by Hering in 1879, to ascertain how the eyes moved during reading. In the previous year, Javal had employed afterimages in his investigations of reading, but this was to demonstrate that the eyes moved horizontally rather than vertically. Hering's and Lamare's auditory method established the discontinuous nature of eye movements during reading, and the photographic methods introduced by Dodge and others in the early twentieth century enabled their characteristics to be determined with greater accuracy. 相似文献
80.
This paper presents a new argument against A-theories of time. A-theorists hold that there is an objective now (present moment)
and an objective flow of time, the latter constituted by the movement of the objective now through time. A-theorists therefore
want to draw different pictures of reality—showing the objective now in different positions—depending upon the time at which
the picture is drawn. In this paper it is argued that the times at which the different pictures are drawn may be taken to
be normal times or hypertimes. If they are normal times then the A-theory is inconsistent, or else collapses to the B-theory—and
appealing to primitive tense operators will not help A-theorists avoid this conclusion. If the times are hypertimes then the
A-theory is consistent, but deeply problematic none the less. 相似文献