全文获取类型
收费全文 | 168篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
172篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 4篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 18篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 6篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 5篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有172条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
Frank E. Pollick Hiroshi Watanabe Mitsuo Kawato 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1996,58(5):762-780
The perception of local orientation from shaded images was examined. In Experiment 1, subjects viewed a boundaryless Gaussian hill and judged local orientation using both a gauge figure and a pointing method. One subject reported an internally consistent surface which was incompatible with the judged light-source direction and model used to generate the image. The remaining subjects reported a surface similar to the generating one, and analysis of their results indicated a contour of zero difference between response and generating slants. This contour of zero slant difference was explored in three subsequent experiments using the pointing technique. These experiments investigated possible influences of luminance artifact (Experiment 2), perception of global orientation (Experiment 3), and self-occluding contours (Experiment 4). All three of these experiments yielded results similar to those of Experiment 1, with distinct contours of zero slant difference. This contour was explored for relationships with the simulated slant of the generating surface and the differential structure of image intensity. This analysis indicated that the contour of zero slant difference was approximately a line of constant slant which shared large regions of adjacency to the zero crossings of the second directional derivative of image intensity. 相似文献
64.
In some cases, perceptual learning is task-specific. However, task-dependent effects of perceptual learning on psychophysical motion-tuning functions have yet to be clarified. In the present study, subjects performed motion detection or discrimination of the same stimulus over the course of four sessions held on separate days. Subjects who performed motion detection showed the most highly improved performance on the trained motion directions. However, after discrimination training, the highest improvement was not observed at the trained directions but shifted away from them. These results can be explained by lateral inhibition. Task demands may differentially modulate excitatory and inhibitory signals to directions in the vicinity of the trained directions. 相似文献
65.
Moriai-Izawa A Dan H Dan I Sano T Oguro K Yokota H Tsuzuki D Watanabe E 《Brain and language》2012,121(3):185-193
Confrontation naming tasks assess cognitive processes involved in the main stage of word production. However, in fMRI, the occurrence of movement artifacts necessitates the use of covert paradigms, which has limited clinical applications. Thus, we explored the feasibility of adopting multichannel functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to assess language function during covert and overt naming tasks. Thirty right-handed, healthy adult volunteers underwent both naming tasks and cortical hemodynamics measurement using fNIRS. The overt naming task recruited the classical left-hemisphere language areas (left inferior frontal, superior and middle temporal, precentral, and postcentral gyri) exemplified by an increase in the oxy-Hb signal. Activations were bilateral in the middle and superior temporal gyri. However, the covert naming task recruited activation only in the left-middle temporal gyrus. The activation patterns reflected a major part of the functional network for overt word production, suggesting the clinical importance of fNIRS in the diagnosis of aphasic patients. 相似文献
66.
We investigated whether implicit learning in a visual search task would influence preferences for visual stimuli. Participants performed a contextual cueing task in which they searched for visual targets, the locations of which were either predicted or not predicted by the positioning of distractors. The speed with which participants located the targets increased across trials more rapidly for predictive displays than for non-predictive displays, consistent with contextual cueing. Participants were subsequently asked to rate the "goodness" of visual displays. The rating results showed that they preferred predictive displays to both non-predictive and novel displays. The participants did not recognize predictive displays any more frequently than they did non-predictive or novel displays. These results suggest that contextual cueing occurred implicitly and that the implicit learning of visual layouts promotes a preference for visual layouts that are predictive of target location. 相似文献
67.
Watanabe S 《Animal cognition》2011,14(6):797-808
Birds have visual cognition as well developed as humans. Sometimes, the birds show visual discrimination similar to humans,
but the birds may use different cues. Previous reports suggest that global configuration cues are salient for humans, whereas
local elemental cues are salient for pigeons. I analyzed the discriminative behavior of pigeons with scrambled images because
scrambled images keep the local elemental cues of the original images but lose the global configuration cues. If pigeons use
local elemental cues, then, they should show transfer of discrimination from the original images to their scrambled images
and also transfer from the scrambled images to their original images. In Experiment I, I trained pigeons on painting style
discrimination (Japanese paintings vs. Western impressionist paintings) using either the original or scrambled images and
found that the pigeons showed bidirectional transfer. In Experiment II, I trained pigeons on discrimination of “good” versus
“bad” paintings using children’s paintings. The birds showed poor transfer from the original images to their scrambled images
and vise versa. Thus, the pigeons discriminated good and bad paintings based mostly on global configuration cues in this case.
These results suggest that the pigeons use different cues for different discriminations. 相似文献
68.
Numerical representations influence temporal processing. Previous studies have consistently shown that larger numbers are perceived to last longer than smaller ones. However, whether this effect is modulated by the absolute or relative magnitudes of the numbers has yet to be fully understood. Here, participants observed single- and double-digit Arabic numerals in separate experimental blocks and reproduced stimulus duration of 600 or 1200 ms. Our results replicated previous findings that the duration of larger numbers was reproduced longer than that of smaller numbers within each digit set. Although the effect of numerical magnitude across single- and double-digit numerals was found when the numerals were presented for 600 ms, the difference was negligible when they were presented for 1200 ms, suggesting that relative magnitude is an important factor in the number–time interaction in the suprasecond range. These results suggest that contextual influence on number–time interaction may depend on the actual stimulus duration. 相似文献
69.
Summary Effects of test-mask similarity on the masking function were examined in two experiments. In Experiment 1, random bar patterns were used as test and mask stimuli. Bars were oriented in 135° oblique direction in test stimuli, and in 135° or 45° oblique direction in mask stimuli. The SOA was varied from 0 to 100 ms (backward masking). In Experiment 2, red and blue random dot patterns were used as both test and mask stimuli, with SOAs of –100 to 100 ms (forward and backward masking). The subject was asked to report the number of bars or dots as quickly as possible. The results of four subjects in one experiment and five in the other indicated that masking effects were generally greater when the test and mask stimuli were the same in orientation or color than when they were different. Slightly asymmetrical U-shaped functions were obtained both in the same and in different (orientation or color) conditions. A two-factor model with a similarity-related symmetrical integration process and a similarity-unrelated asymmetrical interuption process was considered.Experiment 1 was conducted by the first and third authors at Chiba University, and Experiment 2 was performed by the first and second authors at the University of Tokyo 相似文献
70.
When a cross of either static or moving texture is inserted in the intersection of an Ehrenstein figure, we find that the texture and its motion appear to spread outside of the cross. We suggest that these phenomena are analogous to neon color spreading, wherein an interpretation of a transparent surface makes the properties of that surface, which are physically visible only inside the cross, appear to spread over its apparent extent. In our experiments, the texture and the motion of the cross are attributed to the transparent surface. In addition, the spreading changes the apparent aperture within which the texture and its motion are seen, leading to a corresponding change in the apparent direction of motion. 相似文献