全文获取类型
收费全文 | 7967篇 |
免费 | 116篇 |
专业分类
8083篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 91篇 |
2020年 | 135篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 194篇 |
2017年 | 173篇 |
2016年 | 228篇 |
2015年 | 168篇 |
2014年 | 207篇 |
2013年 | 730篇 |
2012年 | 358篇 |
2011年 | 367篇 |
2010年 | 228篇 |
2009年 | 222篇 |
2008年 | 322篇 |
2007年 | 326篇 |
2006年 | 282篇 |
2005年 | 288篇 |
2004年 | 271篇 |
2003年 | 248篇 |
2002年 | 265篇 |
2001年 | 136篇 |
2000年 | 104篇 |
1999年 | 127篇 |
1998年 | 113篇 |
1997年 | 116篇 |
1996年 | 98篇 |
1995年 | 82篇 |
1994年 | 81篇 |
1993年 | 93篇 |
1992年 | 95篇 |
1991年 | 94篇 |
1990年 | 80篇 |
1989年 | 58篇 |
1988年 | 59篇 |
1987年 | 60篇 |
1986年 | 68篇 |
1985年 | 81篇 |
1984年 | 75篇 |
1983年 | 82篇 |
1982年 | 69篇 |
1981年 | 83篇 |
1980年 | 74篇 |
1979年 | 64篇 |
1978年 | 75篇 |
1977年 | 75篇 |
1976年 | 65篇 |
1975年 | 62篇 |
1974年 | 69篇 |
1973年 | 48篇 |
1966年 | 50篇 |
排序方式: 共有8083条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
Beilock SL Jellison WA Rydell RJ McConnell AR Carr TH 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2006,32(8):1059-1071
Recent work suggests that stereotype threat (ST) harms performance by reducing available working memory capacity. Is this the only mechanism by which ST can occur? Three experiments examined ST's impact on expert golf putting, which is not harmed when working memory is reduced but is hurt when attention is allocated to proceduralized processes that normally run outside working memory. Experiment 1 showed that well learned golf putting is susceptible to ST. Experiments 2 and 3 demonstrated that giving expert golfers a secondary task eliminates ST-induced impairment. Distracting attention away from the stereotype-related behavior eliminates the harmful impact of negative stereotype activation. These results are consistent with explicit monitoring theories of choking under pressure, which suggest that performance degradation can occur when too much attention is allocated to processes that usually run more automatically. Thus, ST alters information processing in multiple ways, inducing performance decrements for different reasons in different tasks. 相似文献
132.
This article describes a multidimensional, multivariate, and multilevel approach to the assessment of nicotine withdrawal. In this prospective study, 70 adult smokers assigned to an active or placebo nicotine patch condition completed multiple daily assessments using an electronic diary. Average and individual growth curves were estimated for affective and nonaffective withdrawal symptoms. All symptoms but hunger increased significantly on the quit day and remained elevated for three weeks. Variability in symptom experiences across individuals increased from pre- to post-quit. Relations between symptom reports (e.g., negative affect or craving) and episodic events (e.g., stressful events or seeing someone smoke) changed from pre-quit to post-quit. Pre-quit increases in negative affect and quit-day increases in craving were inversely related to abstinence three months after the quit day, suggesting that anticipatory and immediate reactions to quitting influence success. 相似文献
133.
Previous research has shown that in a three-term spatial reasoning task, the second premise of a German premise pair is especially easy to comprehend if (1) the prepositional object rather than the grammatical subject denotes the given entity, and if (2) the term denoting the given entity precedes the term denoting the new entity. Accordingly, the second premise is easiest to comprehend with noncanonical word order--that is, with the prepositional object in preverbal position denoting the given entity (e.g., To the right of the given object is the new subject). This finding is explained in terms of contextual licensing of noncanonical word order. Here, we discuss and tested two alternative accounts of contextual licensing, given-new and partially ordered set relations (Poset). The given-new account claims that noncanonical word order is licensed by the term denoting the given entity preceding the term denoting the new entity. On the Poset account, noncanonical word order is licensed if the preverbal constituent introduces a new entity that stands in a transitive, irreflexive, and asymmetric relation to a given entity. Comprehension times for second premises with spatial adverbs in four different word orders support both accounts of contextual licensing; Poset licensing was stronger than given-new licensing. 相似文献
134.
148 young adolescents who reported low frustration tolerance also reported more trait anger, state anger, and hostility. 相似文献
135.
The Implicit Association Test (IAT) was used to investigate automatic fear associations in fear of spiders. Fear associations toward spiders were measured among spider fearful and nonfearful participants (Experiment 1) as well as among nonfearfuls and spider enthusiasts (Experiment 2). It was shown that the IAT is sensitive to personal automatic fear associations and therefore distinguishes between high-fearful, nonfearful, and enthusiastic participants. Moreover, implicit spider associations measured by the IAT predicted avoidance behavior beyond self-reports. The results of Experiment 2 provide additional support for the argument that implicit spider associations are different from general stereotypes or knowledge about spiders. 相似文献
136.
M. Thomas Thangaraj 《Reviews in Religion & Theology》2006,13(3):430-431
Book reviewed:
A More Profound Alleluia: Theology and Worship in Harmony , Leanne Van Dyk (ed.), William B. Eerdmans Publishing 2005 (0-8028-2854-X), xx + 155 pp., pb $15.00 相似文献
A More Profound Alleluia: Theology and Worship in Harmony , Leanne Van Dyk (ed.), William B. Eerdmans Publishing 2005 (0-8028-2854-X), xx + 155 pp., pb $15.00 相似文献
137.
138.
The purpose of this study was to evaluate the efficacy of a premarital educational program, with the secondary goal of determining
if the structure (conjoint versus group) and the participants’ gender influenced program effectiveness. Data were collected
from 29 heterosexual couples. Couples were placed in one of three conditions: (1) premarital education in a group format,
(2) premarital education as an individual couple, or (3) the comparison group (no premarital education). The goal of the 8-week
intervention was to improve couples’ readiness and preparation for marriage as measured by the PREP-M. The findings suggested
that the educational program achieved its stated goal.
*The authors would like to acknowledge Aaron Mathney, Dr. Joe Turner, Dr. Kathy Rettig, Sarah DeJean, and Dena Wyum for their
assistance with this project. 相似文献
139.
This study compares the tendency for numerals to elicit spontaneous perceptions of colour or taste (synaesthesia) with the tendency to visualise numbers as occupying particular visuo-spatial configurations (number forms). The prevalence of number forms was found to be significantly higher in synaesthetes experiencing colour compared both to synaesthetes experiencing taste and to control participants lacking any synaesthetic experience. This suggests that the presence of synaesthetic colour sensations enhances the tendency to explicitly represent numbers in a visuo-spatial format although the two symptoms may nevertheless be logically independent (i.e. it is possible to have number forms without colour, and coloured numbers without forms). Number forms are equally common in men and women, unlike previous reports of synaesthesia that have suggested a strong female bias. Individuals who possess a number form are also likely to possess visuo-spatial forms for other ordinal sequences (e.g. days, months, letters) which suggests that it is the ordinal nature of numbers rather than numerical quantity that gives rise to this particular mode of representation. Finally, we also describe some consequences of number forms for performance in a number comparison task. 相似文献
140.
Dube William V. Balsamo Lyn M. Fowler Thomas R. Dickson Chata A. Lombard Kristin M. Tomanari Gerson Y. 《The Psychological record》2006,56(2):233-244
The Psychological Record - This study examined relations between eye movements and accuracy scores in a delayed matching-to-sample procedure with multiple sample stimuli. Four adult humans... 相似文献