首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   668篇
  免费   19篇
  2022年   7篇
  2020年   6篇
  2019年   11篇
  2018年   21篇
  2017年   9篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   10篇
  2013年   62篇
  2012年   23篇
  2011年   28篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   16篇
  2008年   23篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   29篇
  2005年   20篇
  2004年   18篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   12篇
  2000年   22篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   12篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   12篇
  1994年   7篇
  1993年   12篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   9篇
  1990年   12篇
  1989年   9篇
  1988年   12篇
  1987年   6篇
  1986年   8篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   7篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   7篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   6篇
  1978年   6篇
  1975年   10篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   9篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   5篇
  1970年   7篇
  1968年   5篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有687条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
This paper describes a semi-intensive approach to fluency therapy (SIFT). SIFT's rationale and theoretical origins combine both speak-more-fluently and stutter-more-fluently concepts in a cognitive-behavioral approach where the goals of the program are individualized to the needs of each client. Procedural aspects of the program are outlined. Some very preliminary findings from a small pilot study are discussed.  相似文献   
82.
83.
Two experiments investigated how children acquire knowledge of the direct and the indirect object in terms of linguistic marking. This was done in order to test experimentally the psychological validity of a putative linguistic universal which holds that children should expect a marked linguistic form to be the indirect object. In the two experiments, an artificial language which marked these forms was used. This language eliminated the preposition “to” as well as word order as cues in learning the experimental forms. One group heard the indirect object as the marked form in the artificial language, and another group heard the direct object as the marked form. The linguistic theory would predict that the former group would be superior in learning the artificial language. However, using error scores, no difference was found between the two groups. This negative result was also independent of whether a child had achieved mastery of the normal English constructions involving direct and indirect object relationships. Reasons for the failure to find any evidence for this universal are discussed.  相似文献   
84.
Noninstitutionalized retardates and normal children of equal mental age (approximately 8 years) listened to 11 orders of 20 nouns with no instructions to overtly or covertly rehearse. All subjects were in each of three list conditions: (a) when all words were at the same decibel (dB) level, (b) when the 11th item was at a higher dB level, and (c) when the items were repeated at the same dB level using the rehearsal protocol of Rundus (1971). The normals' overall recall was superior to that of the retardates' recall. Repetition of items had no effect on the performance of either group except in the recency portion of the learning curve and on the initial trial for both groups and the fifth trial for the retarded group. The normal group was more susceptible to the yon Restorff effect than was the retarded group. These results were discussed in terms of the changing and different recall strategies of the two groups.  相似文献   
85.
Subjects matched durations of keypresses to loudnesses of pure tones. Resulting duration responses were found to be assimilated to the value of the immediately preceding stimulus and responses from one to at least six trials back in the sequence of loudness stimuli. Responses were contrasted with the values of stimuli two through six trials back in the sequence. These sequential dependencies and other properties of the data were predicted by a cognitive model in which cross-modality matches are mediated by category judgments of stimuli on both continua, and subjects use heuristic strategies to reduce response uncertainty.  相似文献   
86.
Twelve Ss made binary decisions with feedback on numbers from one of two normal distributions with equal variances and unequal means. Sequences of distribution choices corresponded to first-order two-state Markov processes with probabilities of change of state of p1 = p2 = .50, p1 = p2 = .75, and p1 = p2 = .25. Performance was best (in d’ terms) when p1 = p2 ≠ .50. First-order sequential response dependencies tended to mirror the first-order stimulus dependencies. Violations of a fixed cutoff point decision rule were concentrated in the region of the average critical point, with a bandwidth of about 1/2σ, in which violations were strikingly more frequent than would be expected if they had occurred randomly. These results imply that in this task Ss are using a criterion-band decision rule instead of a fixed cutoff point rule, and that they are basing decisions in the region of the criterion band on information extracted from the sequence of decisions presented to them. The average bandwidth is generally different from the optimum bandwidth used by an ideal O in combining the two sources of information.  相似文献   
87.
The purpose of this paper is to describe the use of on-line computers in the teaching of experimental methodology. Computer models are featured.  相似文献   
88.
89.
90.
Consistent relationships are found between Ss′ absolute judgments of the value of a stimulus and the previous sequence of both stimuli and responses. The form and magnitude of these sequential effects are shown to depend on the presence or absence of feedback and on task difficulty. The pattern of the sequential effects found allows the conclusion that they are due to purely response-system processes. A two-stage model of the judgment process is proposed, and it is argued that observed assimilative effects account for the central tendency effects observed in category judgments.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号