首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   183篇
  免费   2篇
  185篇
  2023年   2篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   1篇
  2019年   3篇
  2018年   7篇
  2017年   3篇
  2016年   3篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   38篇
  2012年   8篇
  2011年   11篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   1篇
  2008年   3篇
  2007年   7篇
  2006年   1篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   4篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   7篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   5篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   2篇
  1971年   1篇
  1969年   2篇
  1968年   4篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   2篇
  1960年   2篇
  1959年   1篇
排序方式: 共有185条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
Three hysterical movement disorders   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
132.
The development of the visual placing response was studied in the human infant using an apparatus that controlled the visual stimulus (texture density of patterns) and the depth at which stimuli were presented. The visual placing response was found to be nearly an all-or-none response having an acute onset at between 170 and 190 days of age. The response is identified in the human infant by rigid extension of the hands and arms, characteristically with the fingers fanned. Shortly after it develops, the response appears to be visually mediated and is well differentiated with respect to increasing depth. In contrast, the older infants showed less well differentiated visual placing, tending to respond to all depths equally. The results suggest that there are two phases in the development of the response. The first phase is the initial acquisition of the response and is determined largely by visual depth. The second phase occurs later in development, near the time of walking, and seems to be determined by several factors, including both texture density and depth.  相似文献   
133.
134.
135.
A lickometer for recording dry (unreinforced) tube licking in rats is described. As opposed to the conventional drinkometer, where each lick produces fluid, the lickometer provides the possibility for automatically programmed intermittent reinforcement of (dry) licking, and it does not involve electrical connections through the chamber floor. Comparison of licking and leverpressing with equivalent reinforcers and under equivalent schedules of reinforcement is thus feasible.  相似文献   
136.
The incremental validity of the 4 facet scores (Interpersonal, Affective, Lifestyle, Antisocial) of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R; R. D. Hare, 1991, 2003) and the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version (PCL:SV; S. D. Hart, D. N. Cox, & R. D. Hare, 1995) was evaluated in 6 forensic/correctional samples with average follow-ups ranging from 20 weeks to 10 years. Results indicated that whereas Facet 4 (Antisocial) achieved incremental validity relative to the first 3 facets (Interpersonal, Affective, and Lifestyle) in predicting recidivism in all 6 samples, a block of the first 3 facets achieved incremental validity relative to the 4th facet in only 1 sample. Thus, although there was consistent support for the incremental validity of Facet 4 above and beyond the first 3 facets, there was minimal support for the incremental validity of Facets 1, 2, and 3 above and beyond Facet 4. The implications of these findings for the psychopathy construct in general and the PCL-R/SV in particular are discussed.  相似文献   
137.
A taxometric analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) is conducted on a group of 409 male maximum-, medium-, and minimum-security federal prison inmates using the four PCL-R facet scores (interpersonal, affective, impulsive lifestyle, and antisocial behavior) as indicators. Results obtained from three quasi-independent taxometric procedures-mean above minus below a cut, maximum eigenvalue, and latent mode factor analysis-reveal consistent support for a dimensional interpretation of the psychopathy concept in a moderately heterogeneous sample of participants. On the basis of these results, it is concluded that psychopathy falls along a continuum and that high and low scores on the PCL-R reflect quantitative differences in degree rather than qualitative differences in kind.  相似文献   
138.
Walters GD 《Assessment》2007,14(1):35-43
Four samples were used to evaluate the incremental validity of the Psychological Inventory of Criminal Thinking Styles (PICTS) General Criminal Thinking (GCT) score and Proactive (P) and Reactive (R) composite scales vis-á-vis response style in predicting disciplinary adjustment and recidivism. Purified Confusion (Cf(p)) and Defensiveness (Df(p)) validity scales were constructed by eliminating items from the PICTS Confusion and Defensiveness scales that overlapped with GCT, P, and R. The four-item Cf(p) and Df(p) scales were employed as measures of response style and entered into binomial probit regression analyses alongside GCT, P, and R. The GCT score consistently predicted disciplinary adjustment and recidivism in male and female offenders when included in regression equations with Cf(p) and Df(p). The P scale only displayed incremental validity relative to Cf(p) and Df(p) in large groups of participants, whereas the R scale was incapable of predicting recidivism in female offenders when paired with Cf(p) and Df(p).  相似文献   
139.
    
Moti Mizrahi ( 2013 ) presents some novel counterexamples to Hypothetical Syllogism (HS) for indicative conditionals. I show that they are not compelling as they neglect the complicated ways in which conditionals and modals interact. I then briefly outline why HS should nevertheless be rejected.  相似文献   
140.
    
Throughout the late 1970s and 1980s, Britain, along with other western European countries, experienced structural unemployment on a large scale. Adult education has responded to this new situation with initiatives that embrace ideas of both vocational and liberal traditions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号