首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1260篇
  免费   33篇
  1293篇
  2020年   12篇
  2019年   27篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   15篇
  2015年   15篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   201篇
  2012年   39篇
  2011年   40篇
  2010年   33篇
  2009年   29篇
  2008年   46篇
  2007年   33篇
  2006年   28篇
  2005年   35篇
  2004年   32篇
  2003年   34篇
  2002年   34篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   26篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   10篇
  1995年   11篇
  1994年   11篇
  1993年   15篇
  1992年   16篇
  1991年   13篇
  1990年   14篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   18篇
  1987年   13篇
  1986年   14篇
  1985年   11篇
  1984年   17篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   15篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   16篇
  1979年   18篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   18篇
  1976年   13篇
  1975年   13篇
  1974年   15篇
  1973年   16篇
  1972年   11篇
  1969年   12篇
  1966年   14篇
排序方式: 共有1293条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
One hundred twenty goldfish were differentially trained with large and small reward in a shuttle swimway. The end compartments and the swimming directions for a given S were consistently identifiable over all trials with either a large or small magnitude of reward. Negative contrast effects were obtained in the start measure for Ss with short intervals between trials. The results suggest similar mechanisms for rat and goldfish performance in differential conditioning studies.  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
We tested the hypothesis that differences in attitudes toward conflicting procedures would depend on the context of the presentation and an individual's cognitive style. Data from 182 British school teachers who completed the Kirton (1976) Adaption–Innovation Inventory and two questionnaires concerned with aspects of educational procedures provided results broadly in line with expected adaption–innovation theory. Innovators were found to have a preference for procedures loose in structure, with aims not easily defined, and without easy methods of assessment. The adaptors showed a preference for procedures containing a tighter structure, more definable aims and more precise methods of assessment. The data also provide useful additional norms and information on teachers' cognitive style differences with regard to subject taught, gender, and length of service.  相似文献   
995.
996.
Over the years, research in the social sciences has been dominated by reporting of reliability coefficients that fail to account for key sources of measurement error. Use of these coefficients, in turn, to correct for measurement error can hinder scientific progress by misrepresenting true relationships among the underlying constructs being investigated. In the research reported here, we addressed these issues using generalizability theory (G-theory) in both traditional and new ways to account for the three key sources of measurement error (random-response, specific-factor, and transient) that affect scores from objectively scored measures. Results from 20 widely used measures of personality, self-concept, and socially desirable responding showed that conventional indices consistently misrepresented reliability and relationships among psychological constructs by failing to account for key sources of measurement error and correlated transient errors within occasions. The results further revealed that G-theory served as an effective framework for remedying these problems. We discuss possible extensions in future research and provide code from the computer package R in an online supplement to enable readers to apply the procedures we demonstrate to their own research.  相似文献   
997.
Based on the conceptualization that social desirable bias (SDB) is a discrete event resulting from an interaction between a scale's items, the testing situation, and the respondent's latent trait on a social desirability factor, we present a method that makes use of factor mixture models to identify which examinees are most likely to provide biased responses, which items elicit the most socially desirable responses, and which external variables predict SDB. Problems associated with the common use of correlation coefficients based on scales' total scores to diagnose SDB and partial correlations to correct for SDB are discussed. The method is demonstrated with an analysis of SDB in the Attitude toward Interprofessional Service-Learning scale with a sample of students from health-related fields.  相似文献   
998.
Attention deployment and generating specific types of cognitions are central cognitive mechanisms of emotion regulation. Two groups of hypotheses make contradicting predictions about the emotion-cognition relationship. The moodcongruency hypothesis expects the emergence of mood-congruent cognitions (i.e., negative mood leads to negative and positive mood to positive cognitions). Similarly, a substantial body of research suggests that negative mood induces selffocus, whereas positive mood elicits an external focus of attention. The moodrepair hypothesis, on the other hand, assumes that persons in a negative mood state summon thoughts incongruent with that state and divert attention away from the self. However, the temporal sequence of cognitions assessed as well as coping dispositions, such as vigilance and cognitive avoidance, may moderate these relationships. Positive and negative emotional states were elicited by exposing the participants to the experience of success or failure in a demanding cognitive task. Cognitions that were present after emotion induction were assessed by means of a thought-listing procedure. For the total sample, results clearly confirmed the moodcongruency hypothesis. Thought order was a critical factor only for changes in self-focus. Thought valence (positive, neutral, negative) as well as self-focus were substantially influenced by coping dispositions.  相似文献   
999.
How is admiration different from adoration? We provided one answer to this question by examining the pathways through which admiration and adoration linked to self-expansion in a questionnaire and an experimental (autobiographical recall of emotion episodes) study. Both emotions were associated with increased potential efficacy to accomplish goals (i.e., self-expansion), but different action tendencies accounted for these links. While our emotion inductions did not successfully distinguish between admiration and adoration, we could statistically disentangle their effects through mediator models. In both studies, self-reported admiration linked to self-expansion through the tendency to emulate admired others. Adoration related to self-expansion through the tendency to affiliate with adored others. These findings were obtained after controlling for other emotions in response to the target person (awe, love, hope, benign envy) and mutuality of the relationship. Our findings also suggest that considering specific emotions (rather than undifferentiated positive affect) helps uncover different pathways to self-expansion.  相似文献   
1000.

Quasi-two-dimensional palladium nanoparticles with an average lateral dimension of 7 nm have been prepared by reduction of a PdCl2 graphite intercalation compound precursor by lithium-diphenylide in tetrahydrofuran at room temperature. Selected-area electron diffraction patterns provide evidence that the palladium nanoparticles are hcp single-crystal particles. Owing to the template effect of the graphite lattice, the lattice parameter of palladium was found to have a strong relation with the graphite, and a 3a graphite superstructure was inferred. The palladium structure is rotated by 30o with regard to the carbon host lattice. Raman spectroscopy on this sample showed that a charge transfer between carbon and palladium occurs. The sample can be considered as a common Pd-graphite intercalation compound with palladium nanoparticles as guest. This behaviour is different from palladium nanoparticles prepared by hydrogen reduction at higher temperatures from the same precursor material. These particles may represent an early stage of nanoparticle formation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号