首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   150篇
  免费   7篇
  2021年   1篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   2篇
  2018年   4篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   4篇
  2015年   2篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   19篇
  2012年   4篇
  2011年   7篇
  2010年   4篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   5篇
  2006年   3篇
  2005年   5篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   3篇
  2002年   3篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   2篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   3篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   3篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   3篇
  1986年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   3篇
  1982年   2篇
  1981年   4篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   2篇
  1978年   2篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   2篇
  1974年   2篇
  1973年   3篇
  1972年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
  1969年   2篇
排序方式: 共有157条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Donald Davidson argues that his interpretivist approach to meaning shows that accounting for the intentionality and objectivity of thought does not require an appeal, as John McDowell has urged it does, to a specifically rational relation between mind and world. Moreover, Davidson claims that the idea of such a relation is unintelligible. This paper takes issue with these claims. It shows, first, that interpretivism, contra Davidson's express view, does not depend essentially upon an appeal to a causal relation between events in the world and speakers' beliefs. Second, it shows that interpretivism essentially, if implicitly, depends upon interpreters' appealing to facts taken in in perception, and that such facts are suited to provide a rational connection between mind and world. The paper then argues that none of Davidson's legitimate epistemological arguments tell against the idea that experience, in the form of the propositional contents of perception, can play a role in doxastic economy. Finally, it argues that granting experience such a role is consistent with Davidson's coherentist slogan that nothing can count as a reason for holding a belief except another belief.  相似文献   
82.
The treatment components that contribute to and account for successful therapeutic outcomes for people who stutter are not well understood and are debated by many. The purpose of this phenomenological study was to describe in detail the underlying factors that contribute to a successful or unsuccessful therapeutic interaction between clients and their clinicians. Twenty-eight participants, 19 men and 9 women, who had received from 6 months to more than 12 years of therapy for stuttering were studied. The participants were asked to consider their experience with one or more speech-language pathologists with whom they had received fluency therapy and to describe the characteristics that made that individual effective or ineffective in promoting successful change in their ability to communicate. Analysis of these data resulted in 15 primary categories. Finally, the essential structure of an effective and ineffective therapeutic interaction was described. Results highlighted the importance for effective therapy of understanding the stuttering experience, forming a positive client-clinician, alliance, and being knowledgeable about stuttering and its treatment. Educational objectives: The reader will be able to: (1) describe, from the perspective of a select group of adults who stutter, the themes associated with an effective therapeutic interaction, (2) describe, from the perspective of a select group of adults who stutter, the themes associated with an ineffective therapeutic interaction, and (3) describe the ways in which an effective or ineffective therapeutic interaction could impact a person who stutters.  相似文献   
83.
According to Michel Serres, and those who support his views about education, the truly educated person is one who has been exposed to the so-called Third Curriculum. This conviction is evaluated from the vantage point of a Christian view of reality, education, and the curriculum. The author concludes that while Third-Curriculum thinking appears to be innovative and creative, its two central tenets of interdisciplinarity and dialogical thinking are not quite so new. He also concludes that the Third-Curriculum theory is still too vague, inconsistent, and programmatic to be seriously considered for implementation in schools. Its postmodern refusal to acknowledge the divine, law-subjected orderliness of creation as well as its tendency to reduce the God-given diversity in reality to only one or two aspects of reality are contrary to convictions underlying the Christian view of the curriculum and curriculum design.  相似文献   
84.
An analysis of recent publications in the field of school effectiveness studies shows that virtually no contributions have been made in that knowledge domain with respect to the effectiveness of Christian schools. To address this shortcoming—while keeping the undesirability of a package approach to Christian education in mind—a philosophical-pedagogical approach to a more systematic assessment of Christian schools is offered. A number of other considerations regarding the assessment of the effectiveness of Christian schools are left for further discussion and investigation.  相似文献   
85.
African American youth are less likely to use alcohol than their European American counterparts; however, the greater consequences of use for African American youth highlight the need for greater research attention to this group. Two social contexts that have been linked with adolescent alcohol use are parents and peers, yet these studies have rarely included African American youth or failed to examine potential racial differences. This study examined the main and interactive effects of parents and peers, as well as the moderating role of race on alcohol use, in African American and European American rural adolescents (n=71) identified as at high-risk for alcohol use. Contrary to study hypotheses, however, parents were not a more robust moderator for African American than European American youth. Clinical implications for prevention and intervention programming for both African American and European American youth are discussed.  相似文献   
86.
To examine whether explaining hypothetical childhood events makes people more confident that the events really happened, we used a method similar to the imagination inflation procedure devised by Garry, Manning, Loftus, and Sherman (1996). First, participants rated how confident they were that a list of events occurred in their childhood. Two weeks later, participants explained how four of the eight target events could have happened to them before the age of 10. Finally, participants made confidence ratings about the list of childhood events for a second time. Participants were more confident that the explained hypothetical childhood events really did happen compared to events they did not explain. We discuss both theoretical and practical implications of these results. Copyright © 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
87.
The concepts of locus of control and locus of causality are similar and refer to the degree to which a person perceives daily occurrences to be a consequence of his or her own behavior. Locus of control is considered to be a unidimensional construct indicating an inverse relationship between the polls of internality and externality. The locus of control is generally determined by using questionnaires with a limited number of items. Locus of causality is considered to be a two-dimensional construct where Origin and Pawn values, which are similar to internality and externality, respectively, are not necessarily inversely related. Locus of causality is determined by content analysis of freely spoken or written narratives. In the current study Origin and Pawn scores were obtained from twenty adults prior to and following a three-week intensive stuttering treatment program. Brief narratives written by the participants were analyzed to obtain Origin and Pawn values. These scores were compared with traditional measures of therapeutic outcome (Locus of Control, OASES, PSI, percentage of syllables stuttered). Results indicated statistically significant increases in pre- to post-treatment Origin scores (p = .001; Cohen's d = 1.44) and statistically significant decreases in pre- to post-treatment Pawn scores (p = .003; Cohen's d = 1.11). Origin and Pawn scores showed significant relationships with other measures of stuttering, indicating concurrent and construct validity. Origin and Pawn scaling procedures appear to provide a valid, sensitive, and nonreactive indicator of the speaker's locus of causality and ability to develop an autonomous and agentic lifestyle.Educational objectives: After reading this article, the readers will be able to: (1) distinguish between the concepts of locus of control and locus of causality, (2) describe the characteristics of individuals behaving as an Origin and a Pawn, (3) differentiate patterns of change for Origin and Pawn scores prior to and following treatment, and (4) describe the clinical advantages using Origin and Pawn scaling procedures for individuals who stutter.  相似文献   
88.
89.
90.
This article argues that an iconic event in the history of helping research -- the story of the 38 witnesses who remained inactive during the murder of Kitty Genovese -- is not supported by the available evidence. Using archive material, the authors show that there is no evidence for the presence of 38 witnesses, or that witnesses observed the murder, or that witnesses remained inactive. Drawing a distinction between the robust bystander research tradition and the story of the 38 witnesses, the authors explore the consequences of the story for the discipline of psychology. They argue that the story itself plays a key role in psychology textbooks. They also suggest that the story marks a new way of conceptualizing the dangers of immersion in social groups. Finally, they suggest that the story itself has become a modern parable, the telling of which has served to limit the scope of inquiry into emergency helping.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号