全文获取类型
收费全文 | 720篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
729篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 15篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 26篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 15篇 |
2013年 | 87篇 |
2012年 | 26篇 |
2011年 | 30篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 15篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 16篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 22篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 13篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有729条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
Norman M. Kiracofe Patsy A. Donn Charles O. Grant Edward E. Podolnick Rosie P. Bingham Herbert R. Bolland Clarke G. Carney Judith Clementson Robert P. Gallagher Richard D. Grosz Lee Handy Janet H. Hansche Judith K. Mack Donald Sanz Lilly J. Walker Kerry T. Yamada 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1994,73(1):38-43
The accreditation standards outlined in the article are used by the International Association of Counseling Services, Inc., as the basis for the formal accreditation of college and university counseling programs throughout the United States and Canada. They reflect the program elements and practice standards that are deemed essential in a counseling center that provides high-quality services to students. 相似文献
42.
Judy Garber Deborah A. Van Slyke Lynn S. Walker 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1998,26(5):381-391
Mother–child concordance regarding children's somatic and emotional symptoms was assessed in children with recurrent abdominal pain (n = 88), emotional disorders (n = 51), and well children (n = 56). Children between 6 and 18 years of age and their mothers completed questionnaires assessing the children's somatic symptoms, functional disability, and depression. Mothers of children with recurrent abdominal pain reported more child somatic and depressive symptoms than did their children, and mothers of children with emotional disorders reported more child depressive symptoms than did their children. Higher levels of maternal distress were associated with greater mother-child discordance in the direction of mothers reporting more child symptoms than did their children. No significant child age or sex differences were found in concordance patterns. 相似文献
43.
44.
45.
46.
‘Contract cheating’ has recently emerged as a form of academic dishonesty. It involves students contracting out their coursework
to writers in order to submit the purchased assignments as their own work, usually via the internet. This form of cheating
involves epistemic and ethical problems that are continuous with older forms of cheating, but which it also casts in a new
form. It is a concern to educators because it is very difficult to detect, because it is arguably more fraudulent than some
other forms of plagiarism, and because it appears to be connected to a range of systemic problems within modern higher education.
This paper provides an overview of the information and literature thus far available on the topic, including its definition,
the problems it involves, its causal factors, and the ways in which educators might respond. We argue that while contract
cheating is a concern, some of the suggested responses are themselves problematic, and that best practice responses to the
issue should avoid moral panic and remain focussed on supporting honest students and good academic practice. 相似文献
47.
J. Bret Becton Jon C. Carr Kevin W. Mossholder H. Jack Walker 《Journal of business and psychology》2017,32(4):495-508
Purpose
The purpose of this study was to investigate the performance–turnover relationship by considering the effects of task performance and OCBs simultaneously while also examining the moderating effect job complexity has on the relationship between voluntary turnover and each type of performance.Design/Methodology/Approach
Data were obtained as part of a larger study to validate an employment test, in which actual turnover data and supervisory ratings of job performance were collected for employees in two hospitals (n = 782).Findings
Task performance exhibited a curvilinear relationship with turnover, while OCB exhibited a negative linear relationship with turnover. Job complexity moderated both of these relationships. For task performance, turnover in high-complexity jobs was greater for low performers but lower for high performers relative to that of employees in low-complexity jobs. For OCB, the negative relationship with turnover was more pronounced in high-complexity jobs.Implications
Both low- and high-task performers are more likely to turnover, while employees exhibiting high OCBs are less likely to turnover. These results imply that retention strategies are critical for top performers, but especially in high-complexity jobs. Organizations may be able to discourage voluntary turnover by creating conditions that stimulate OCB, particularly in highly complex jobs.Originality/Value
Most prior performance–turnover relationship research used unidimensional measures of performance, whereas this study included two dimensions of performance and examined this relationship while controlling for one-performance dimension when predicting the other. Furthermore, this study is one of the first studies to suggest that job complexity moderates the performance–turnover relationship.48.
Elder JH Donaldson SO Kairalla J Valcante G Bendixen R Ferdig R Self E Walker J Palau C Serrano M 《Journal of child and family studies》2011,20(3):263-271
Literature regarding fathers of children with autism remains sparse, and because mothers are the more common intervening parent,
few training methods have focused on fathers. Thus, we sought to evaluate effects of in-home training directed at fathers
and their ability to train mothers in the same manner in which they were trained. Fathers were taught four skills commonly
associated with in-home training interventions for parents of children with autism: following the child’s lead, imitation
with animation, commenting on the child, and expectant waiting. Father skills were evaluated twice a week for 12 weeks during
videotaped in-home father–child play sessions. Analyses included visual inspection of graphed data and statistical analyses
of father skill acquisition, mother skill acquisition, and child behaviors with both parents. A multivariate repeated measures
analysis of 18 dyads revealed significant increases in frequencies of fathers’ imitation with animation, expectant waiting,
and commenting on the child. Child initiating rates increased significantly as did frequencies of child non-speech vocalizations.
Analysis of mothers revealed significant increases in frequencies of imitation with animation, expectant waiting, and following
the child’s lead. Child behaviors had similar results for father and mother sessions. Findings are consistent with those from
our first study indicating that fathers can effectively implement skills that promote father–child social interactions and
that children respond positively to this approach. 相似文献
49.
Bettina Rolke Susana Ruiz Fernández Mareike Schmid Matthias Walker Martin Lachmair Juan José Rahona López Gonzalo Hervás Carmelo Vázquez 《Cognitive processing》2013,14(3):231-244
The notion of a mental time-line (i.e., past corresponds to left and future corresponds to right) supports the conceptual metaphor view assuming that abstract concepts like “time” are grounded in cognitively more accessible concepts like “space.” In five experiments, we further investigated the relationship between temporal and spatial representations and examined whether or not the spatial correspondents of time are unintentionally activated. We employed a priming paradigm, in which visual or auditory prime words (i.e., temporal adverbs such as yesterday, tomorrow) preceded a colored square. In all experiments, participants discriminated the color of this square by responding with the left or the right hand. Although the temporal reference of the priming adverb was task irrelevant in Experiment 1, visually presented primes facilitated responses to the square in correspondence with the direction of the mental time-line. This priming effect was absent in Experiments 2, 3, and 5, in which the primes were presented auditorily and the temporal reference of the words could be ignored. The effect, however, emerged when attention was oriented to the temporal content of the auditory prime words in Experiment 4. The results suggest that task demands differentially modulate the activation of the mental time-line within the visual and auditory modality and support a flexible association between conceptual codes. 相似文献
50.
Walker RL 《Theoretical medicine and bioethics》2006,27(4):305-331
Human beings with diminished decision-making capacities are usually thought to require greater protections from the potential harms of research than fully autonomous persons. Animal subjects of research receive lesser protections than any human beings regardless of decision-making capacity. Paradoxically, however, it is precisely animals’ lack of some characteristic human capacities that is commonly invoked to justify using them for human purposes. In other words, for humans lesser capacities correspond to greater protections but for animals the opposite is true. Without explicit justification, it is not clear why or whether this should be the case. Ethics regulations guiding human subject research include principles such as respect for persons—and related duties—that are required as a matter of justice while regulations guiding animal subject research attend only to highly circumscribed considerations of welfare. Further, the regulations guiding research on animals discount any consideration of animal welfare relative to comparable human welfare. This paper explores two of the most promising justifications for these differences␣between the two sets of regulations. The first potential justification points to lesser moral status for animals on the basis of their lesser capacities. The second potential justification relies on a claim about the permissibility of moral partiality as␣found in common morality. While neither potential justification is sufficient to justify the regulatory difference as it stands, there is possible common ground between supporters of some regulatory difference and those rejecting the current difference. 相似文献