全文获取类型
收费全文 | 22737篇 |
免费 | 751篇 |
国内免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
23499篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 206篇 |
2018年 | 315篇 |
2017年 | 360篇 |
2016年 | 357篇 |
2015年 | 230篇 |
2014年 | 302篇 |
2013年 | 1644篇 |
2012年 | 605篇 |
2011年 | 656篇 |
2010年 | 409篇 |
2009年 | 382篇 |
2008年 | 563篇 |
2007年 | 551篇 |
2006年 | 500篇 |
2005年 | 456篇 |
2004年 | 438篇 |
2003年 | 401篇 |
2002年 | 455篇 |
2001年 | 723篇 |
2000年 | 748篇 |
1999年 | 555篇 |
1998年 | 256篇 |
1997年 | 220篇 |
1996年 | 205篇 |
1992年 | 462篇 |
1991年 | 423篇 |
1990年 | 452篇 |
1989年 | 428篇 |
1988年 | 403篇 |
1987年 | 408篇 |
1986年 | 419篇 |
1985年 | 440篇 |
1984年 | 347篇 |
1983年 | 321篇 |
1982年 | 243篇 |
1981年 | 231篇 |
1979年 | 397篇 |
1978年 | 293篇 |
1977年 | 260篇 |
1976年 | 234篇 |
1975年 | 349篇 |
1974年 | 384篇 |
1973年 | 364篇 |
1972年 | 333篇 |
1971年 | 297篇 |
1970年 | 276篇 |
1969年 | 325篇 |
1968年 | 362篇 |
1967年 | 330篇 |
1966年 | 327篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Stress, locus of control, and achievement of high school seniors 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
73.
74.
Practice in front of a mirror is a common procedure for activities such as dance, gymnastics, and other sports. The purpose of this study was to examine the effect that performing with concurrent visual feedback from a mirror had on the acquisition of the power clean movement. 18 college-age males who had no prior experience with the power clean movement served as subjects who were assigned to one of two groups. One group had use of a mirror during the practice trials and the other practiced without the mirror. All subjects viewed an instructional videotape and had practice trials. All subjects were evaluated for proper technique on a pretest, a posttest without the mirror, and a posttest with the mirror. Analysis showed a significant difference between pre- and posttest performances for both groups and a significant difference between groups on the posttest performances with the mirror. Evidently the videotaped instruction was sufficient to allow both groups to improve in performance of the power clean. Differences in posttest performances with the mirror reflected the type of feedback (with or without the mirror) available during training. 相似文献
75.
The present study investigates the effect of spatial orientation of the body on heart rate during conditions of water-immersion sensory isolation. Two groups of three men and three women each were exposed to one of two isolation conditions. In one condition the subject was suspended in a buoyant but vertical attitude; in a second condition the subject was suspended in a buoyant but horizontal attitude. Heart-rate data were obtained continuously during each session. There was no significant difference in mean heart rate between the two conditions. The data were interpreted as providing support for the position that studies comparing recumbent subjects in air isolation with underwater subjects in a vertical suspension attitude are valid, at least for some measures, and are not an artifact of spatial bodily orientation. The possible importance of neutral buoyancy in accounting for the nonsignificant effect of body attitude on heart rate is discussed. 相似文献
76.
Multiunit activity (MUA) was recorded chronically in the hippocampus (CA3) and the medial geniculate body (mMG) during habituation to a tone followed by conditioning (tone paired with footshock) or pseudoconditioning (tone/footshock unpaired) in rats previously trained in a lever-pressing for food task (VI 60). In the conditioned group pairing tones with footshocks rapidly induced an increase in the initial CS-evoked response in the mMG, followed by the emergence of a hippocampal response and a marked conditioned suppression of lever-pressing to the tone. In contrast, in the pseudoconditioned group, the stimulus induced only transient cellular changes in the hippocampus and in the mMG, while no behavioral suppression to the tone could be seen. Moreover, presentations of the CS 45 days later induced multiunit and behavioral responses in both structures, only in the conditioned group. These results are used for discussion of the role of learning-induced changes in the sensory structure (mMG) as compared with changes in an associative structure (hippocampus), during acquisition and retention of a conditioned response. 相似文献
77.
For 3 consecutive days cockroaches (Periplaneta americana) were exposed to either escapable, inescapable, or no shock in an escape task. Twenty-four hours later they were tested in a shuttlebox escape task. There were reliable differences between escapable and inescapable animals and between inescapable and control animals in both escape latencies and the number of failures to escape. 相似文献
78.
Sprague-Dawley rats drank sweetened (3% dextrose + 0.144% saccharin, w/v) or unflavored water for 18 days and subsequent pain reactivity was assessed using a hot plate. Compared to the rats that consumed unflavored water, the rats that consumed sweet water responded more quickly on the hot plate indicating that their threshold for pain was lowered. Another group of rats given identical exposure to the fluids had their brains prepared for measuring opiate receptor binding using the delta-receptor ligand [3H]D-Ala-D-Leu-enkephalin ([3H]DADLE) and the mu-receptor selective ligand [3H]Tyr-D-Ala-Gly-MePhe-Gly-ol ([ 3H]DAGO). Binding of these opiates to mu- and delta-receptors in the cerebral cortex, striatum, hippocampus, hypothalamus, brain stem, and remaining brain regions was the same for the rats that drank sweet fluids and those that drank unflavored water. These findings suggest that drinking sweet fluids lowers pain thresholds but does not alter mu- and delta-receptors. 相似文献
79.
Interactions between area and numerosity 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Piet G. Vos Michiel P. van Oeffelen Hein J. Tibosch Jüri Allik 《Psychological research》1988,50(3):148-154
Summary The concept of filled area, i.e. the impressionistic ensemble of parts of a stimulus field occupied by dots, is used to account for various kinds of numerosity illusions due to perceptual interaction between a number of dots and their spatial arrangement. A measure of filled area is derived from a model for the perceptual clustering of dots on the basis of relative proximity. It is shown that the quantified concept of filled area successfully predicts illusion data from earlier studies. Subsequently, a two-alternatives forced-choice numerosity experiment is reported, the data of which gave further evidence of the predictive power of the filled-area hypothesis. Our approach is discussed with respect to spatial features of dot stimuli that might be in rivalry with the filled-area factor in numerosity-estimation tasks. 相似文献
80.
Twenty trainees and 18 partners took part in a study of the effects of a marriage guidance training programme on their own marriage. Changes were assessed in two ways: retrospectively ('perceived' changes as assessed following training) and prospectively (changes calculated by comparing scores obtained before training and those obtained after training). For most dimensions there was little correspondence between 'perceived' change and 'calculated' change. This has important methodological implications, which are discussed. The 'calculated' change indicated poorer marital adjustment following training. The trainees' scores suggested that they had become less 'abasing' and less 'nurturant' towards their partners; also that they had more disagreements about 'philosophy of life' and 'dealing with in-laws'. Their comments suggested that a number of the trainees had become more analytical of their own marriages, and that training had heightened their awareness of their own needs and individuality. 相似文献