全文获取类型
收费全文 | 28128篇 |
免费 | 1050篇 |
国内免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
29188篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 290篇 |
2018年 | 408篇 |
2017年 | 464篇 |
2016年 | 491篇 |
2015年 | 328篇 |
2014年 | 416篇 |
2013年 | 2096篇 |
2012年 | 720篇 |
2011年 | 794篇 |
2010年 | 538篇 |
2009年 | 507篇 |
2008年 | 772篇 |
2007年 | 695篇 |
2006年 | 689篇 |
2005年 | 578篇 |
2004年 | 581篇 |
2003年 | 603篇 |
2002年 | 596篇 |
2001年 | 699篇 |
2000年 | 700篇 |
1999年 | 541篇 |
1998年 | 298篇 |
1997年 | 316篇 |
1996年 | 316篇 |
1995年 | 292篇 |
1992年 | 519篇 |
1991年 | 486篇 |
1990年 | 484篇 |
1989年 | 451篇 |
1988年 | 482篇 |
1987年 | 435篇 |
1986年 | 473篇 |
1985年 | 474篇 |
1984年 | 417篇 |
1983年 | 370篇 |
1981年 | 294篇 |
1979年 | 432篇 |
1978年 | 343篇 |
1977年 | 304篇 |
1976年 | 321篇 |
1975年 | 361篇 |
1974年 | 452篇 |
1973年 | 496篇 |
1972年 | 355篇 |
1971年 | 376篇 |
1970年 | 338篇 |
1969年 | 368篇 |
1968年 | 448篇 |
1967年 | 411篇 |
1966年 | 424篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
991.
The present research investigated the effects of experimentally induced positive and negative feedback about the self on the recipient's subsequent reactions to aid. In line with a consistency prediction, subjects given positive feedback who later received aid had lower mood ratings and less favorable self-evaluations than those who were given positive feedback but no subsequent help. Conversely, subjects given negative feedback who later received aid had higher mood ratings and more positive self-evaluations than those who were given negative feedback but no subsequent help. Subjects' evaluations of another individual were characterized by aid main effects, which indicate that an other who helped was evaluated more favorably than an other who did not help. The conceptual and applied implications of these results are discussed. 相似文献
992.
993.
The Pollyanna Principle states that people process pleasant information more accurately and efficiently than less pleasant information. This study examined whether different measures of Pollyanna tendencies are correlated with each other. Fourteen measures of Pollyannaism were derived for 133 students. The results showed that subjects who rated themselves as optimistic or happy also showed Pollyannaism on other measures of happiness, believed that the events in their lives were pleasant, gave themselves positive ratings on personality characteristics, recalled pleasant words more often than unpleasant words, supplied more free associations to pleasant stimuli than to unpleasant stimuli, listed pleasant items first, and judged that pleasant words were more frequent in the English language. 相似文献
994.
Three properties of the cusp catastrophe: bimodality, inaccessibility, and hysteresis were evaluated in rabbit eyelid conditioning. Bimodality was evidenced by the fact that responding tended to occur at either very low or very high levels, and inaccessibility by the fact that intermediate levels of responding were rarely encountered. Hysteresis was demonstrated when: (a) levels of responding attained under a continuous schedule of reinforcement were largely maintained by a schedule which was too lean to promote conditioning by itself; and (b) levels of responding attained with a high intensity unconditional stimulus were largely maintained by a lower intensity unconditional stimulus which was too weak to promote conditioning in its own right. These results are consistent with the model of conditioning suggested by P. W. Frey and R. J. Sears (Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 321–340) which postulates response-mapping rules based on the cusp catastrophe. 相似文献
995.
Six dairy cows (Bos taurus) were trained on several pairs of concurrent variable-interval schedules with different types of food available on each alternative. The required response was a plate press made by the animal's muzzle. Performance generally replicated that found with other species. The generalized matching law accounted for the preference data, showing that food preference could be quantitatively analyzed as a special case of response bias. The preference functions showed that the response- and time-allocation ratios were not as extreme as obtained reinforcement rate ratios (undermatching). 相似文献
996.
Five pigeons were trained on concurrent variable-interval schedules. A series of conditions in which the ratio of reinforcement rates on two keys was progressively increased and then decreased was arranged twice. The birds were then exposed to an irregular sequence of conditions. Each condition in which reinforcement was available on both keys lasted six sessions. Performance in the first, third, and sixth sessions after a condition change was analyzed. Following a condition change, preference was biased toward the preference in the last condition, but this effect largely disappeared before the sixth session of training. The birds' preferences also appeared less sensitive to reinforcement rates in early sessions after a transition. Preference in a session was a function of both the reinforcements in that session and the reinforcements obtained in as many as four or five previous sessions. The effects of reinforcements in previous sessions could be summarized by the performance in the immediately preceding session, giving a relatively simple relation between present performance and a combination of present reinforcement and prior session performance. While such hysteresis could cause undermatching when only a small number of sessions are arranged in a condition, undermatching in a stable-state performance probably arises elsewhere. 相似文献
997.
J L?ssner H Bachmann D Biesold K Günther U Ruchholtz W Storch A Wagner 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1979,31(9):513-524
The Leipzig Center for Wilson's Disease in the GDR is charged with the registration and diagnosis of all homozygous Wilson gene carriers, the clarification of all suspected cases, including the heterozygote test, and the co-ordination of long-term treatment. At present, there are 78 recorded Wilson gene carriers living. On the basis of our own comprehensive observations and investigations over prolonged periods of time, questions concerning pathogenesis, genetics, diagnosis and therapeutic measures, including their side-effects, are dealt with. 相似文献
998.
W A Myers 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1979,48(2):292-307
Case material is presented to illustrate the thesis that the ability to create an imaginary companion during childhood is an early expression of the special ego aptitudes found in creative individuals in adult life. Such "companions" allow these children to attempt to master creatively a variety of narcissistic mortifications suffered in reality and to displace unacceptable affects. In creative adults who had imaginary companions in childhood, the early fantasies serve as an organizing schema in memory for the childhood traumata. Stimuli in adult life which evoke the earlier traumata may revive the original imaginary companion fantasies. These then serve as nodal bases for the creation of specific adult works of art. 相似文献
999.
W W Meissner 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1979,48(4):587-600
Freud's account of his meeting with the country maid, Katharina, is re-evaluated from a contemporary psychoanalytic perspective. Freud's original explanation of Katharina's hysteria was based on a set of quantitative-economic assumptions and a psychic model based on conflict and defense. A modern analytic perspective would shift the emphasis from the economics of discharge to the aims and objects of sexual activity. The understanding of sensual pleasure focuses more specifically on the related complex of intentions, purposes, meanings, and motives, as well as on the qualities, characteristics, and patterns of interaction with important objects. 相似文献
1000.
J W Slap 《The Psychoanalytic quarterly》1979,48(4):620-627
While phase-related traumata are often accepted as sufficient explanation of patients' reporting that they feel like "nothing" or "nobody," it is the thesis of this paper that such feelings often derive from intrapsychic conflict and that the working through of such conflict leads to significant therapeutic benefit. In support of this thesis, two cases are presented, together with an addendum on Hogarth's Mr. Nobody. 相似文献