全文获取类型
收费全文 | 54882篇 |
免费 | 1048篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
55933篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 629篇 |
2018年 | 851篇 |
2017年 | 929篇 |
2016年 | 914篇 |
2015年 | 668篇 |
2014年 | 787篇 |
2013年 | 4015篇 |
2012年 | 1403篇 |
2011年 | 1549篇 |
2010年 | 1054篇 |
2009年 | 1000篇 |
2008年 | 1418篇 |
2007年 | 1314篇 |
2006年 | 1246篇 |
2005年 | 1074篇 |
2004年 | 1009篇 |
2003年 | 1060篇 |
2002年 | 1035篇 |
2001年 | 1735篇 |
2000年 | 1679篇 |
1999年 | 1268篇 |
1998年 | 610篇 |
1997年 | 556篇 |
1996年 | 535篇 |
1995年 | 523篇 |
1992年 | 1102篇 |
1991年 | 1066篇 |
1990年 | 1012篇 |
1989年 | 957篇 |
1988年 | 945篇 |
1987年 | 918篇 |
1986年 | 924篇 |
1985年 | 970篇 |
1984年 | 787篇 |
1983年 | 666篇 |
1981年 | 545篇 |
1979年 | 825篇 |
1978年 | 628篇 |
1977年 | 587篇 |
1976年 | 568篇 |
1975年 | 720篇 |
1974年 | 812篇 |
1973年 | 892篇 |
1972年 | 688篇 |
1971年 | 655篇 |
1970年 | 608篇 |
1969年 | 650篇 |
1968年 | 770篇 |
1967年 | 697篇 |
1966年 | 751篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
Sydney S. Zentall Arlene M. Hall David L. Lee 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1998,26(5):335-343
Mirrors have been used to focus attention to aspects of the self (e.g., to known strategies, standards). We hypothesized that this could be important for students with hyperactivity/inattention, who typically direct attention outward to external novelty. In this study, we administered a partially solvable word puzzle to 43 middle school students, with and without hyperactivity/inattention, in the presence and absence of a mirror, counterbalanced for condition and form order. Differences between students with hyperactivity/inattention and comparisons in accuracy were found only in the no mirror condition. Furthermore, the beneficial effect of the mirror for children with hyperactivity/inattention was most pronounced for those who looked at the mirror. Findings were interpreted in terms of their potential to remedy the production deficits of these children. 相似文献
822.
This paper describes the development and validation of a personnel selection instrument designed to predict employee tenure. The instrument was developed using models of employee turnover, psychodynamic theory, and beliefs regarding the optimal way to advance one's career. Four predictive designs were conducted to validate the instrument. Correlation coefficients ranged from .14 (p < .07) to .28 (p < .05) in predicting tenure. 相似文献
823.
As researchers become more interested instudying the influences of the male gender role onpeople's physical and psychological well-being, measuresof several male role domains have been created,including psychological agency, traditional attitudestowards men, and masculine gender role stress (MGRS).These measures of the male gender role, however,typically have been constructed and validated using only samples of men. This restriction introducesquestions about the reliability and validity of theseinstruments in samples of women, leading to problemswhen generalizing findings to this group. The present study addressed these issues by examining thefactor structure, factor correlations, and latent meansof these three male role measures for a sample of mostlyCaucasian men and women, using multiple group confirmatory factor analytic techniques.Results showed only the dimensionality of the agenticpersonality traits differed significantly for men andwomen. The interrelationships among and latent means of agency, attitudes, and MGRS differed by sex.These findings demonstrate the utility of the currentconceptualization of sex and gender; in other words,that gender is not totally determined by sexand that men and women internalize many of the samegender cues, but differentially endorse them to varyingdegrees. 相似文献
824.
J. F. Dmonet J. A. Fiez E. Paulesu S. E. Petersen R. J. Zatorre 《Brain and language》1996,55(3):352-379
Poeppel (1996) raises a number of criticisms about the methods and reported results for eight studies of phonological processing from six different neuroimaging laboratories. We would freely admit that valid criticisms of PET methodology can be made and that, like any method, it has limitations; in fact, we and others have engaged in such critical commentary (Steinmetz & Seitz, 1991; Sergent et al., 1992; Démonet, 1995; Fiez et al., 1996a; Zatorre et al., 1996). Poeppel's analysis, though, falls far short of providing new insights into the limitations of PET methodology or the means by which future functional imaging studies could be improved. Many of Poeppel's criticisms derive from a failure to understand some of the fundamental issues which motivate functional imaging studies, including those he reviews. However, we are grateful to our critic inasmuch as he offers us the challenge to clarify our positions on important aspects of our experimental design, analysis, and interpretation. In our discussion of these issues, we begin with a general commentary, followed by specific comments from individual authors. 相似文献
825.
Peter F. W. PreeceKenneth L. Q. Read 《Intelligence》1996,23(3):229
Based on the construct of classical IQ (mental age/chronological age), a model of the proportions of the population at various stages of cognitive development as a function of age is proposed. The model, which accounts well for empirical data for the age range 13 through 17 years, is shown to compare favorably with other theoretical models. The success of the model provides further evidence of the salience of the construct of general cognitive capacity in differential psychology. 相似文献
826.
Major sources of stress in Hong Kong teachers were investigated, with specific reference to guidance work as a potential source of stress. A survey of 415 secondary school teachers revealed that guidance-related aspects of work constituted a major dimension of stress, with guidance teachers, female teachers, younger teachers and junior teachers perceiving more stress than their counterparts in this dimension. Workload and time pressure were perceived as the most stressful aspects of work for all teachers. Results also showed an association between stress and job satisfaction, and biographical variables. Implications of the findings for teachers doing guidance work in Hong Kong and internationally are discussed. 相似文献
827.
Will A. C. Spijkers Petra Wallmeyer Jos J. Adam Fred G. W. C. Paas Martinus J. Buekers Irina J. Wuyts 《Psychological research》1996,59(2):107-118
Two experiments are reported in which, by means of a pointing task, we studied the stimulus-position effect, i.e. the inverted U-shape form of the reaction-time function in relation to stimulus position in tasks in which stimuli and/or responses are arranged in a horizontal array. The response consisted of aiming the index finger from a central starting point at a target area on a screen. Reaction time was the main dependent variable. The spatial relation between the position of the imperative signal and the position of the response was manipulated by varying the spatial S-R compatibility and physical distance that separated the positions of stimulus and response. The stimulus-position effect was shown to depend on the compatibility of the S-R relation (Exp. 1). In Exp. 2 it was found that the modulation of the stimulus-position effect by spatial compatibility disappeared completely when the distance between the positions of stimulus and response was reduced. None of the experiments revealed that the stimulus position effect depended on signal discriminability, which renders an interpretation of this effect in terms of perceptual processes unlikely. We argue that the attentional model of spatial coding provides the most reasonable explanation of the obtained reaction-time patterns. 相似文献
828.
829.
I.W.R. Bushnell 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》1996,4(1):12-17
Few British organizations utilize handwriting analysis for personality assessment, but in some European countries handwriting analysis is extremely popular. Research has examined the reliability and validity of both methods of assessment, but few studies have directly compared the two. In this study, the personality of 120 subjects was assessed by the Cattell 16PF and by handwriting analysis. Each subject was presented with five handwriting analysis textual reports and five personality textual reports (one of each being their own) and asked to rank order each set in terms of perceived accuracy. The same ranking process was undertaken by each respondent's social partner. The results demonstrated that handwriting reports were ranked at a chance level by Self and by Other and that personality reports were ranked at a well above chance level by Self and by Other. Self-rankings were more accurate than Other-rankings. 相似文献
830.