首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   61955篇
  免费   2415篇
  国内免费   25篇
  2020年   601篇
  2019年   649篇
  2018年   956篇
  2017年   990篇
  2016年   1058篇
  2015年   788篇
  2014年   930篇
  2013年   4449篇
  2012年   1744篇
  2011年   1736篇
  2010年   1134篇
  2009年   1066篇
  2008年   1600篇
  2007年   1544篇
  2006年   1363篇
  2005年   1252篇
  2004年   1193篇
  2003年   1134篇
  2002年   1167篇
  2001年   1817篇
  2000年   1777篇
  1999年   1336篇
  1998年   644篇
  1997年   605篇
  1996年   590篇
  1992年   1239篇
  1991年   1181篇
  1990年   1187篇
  1989年   1060篇
  1988年   1057篇
  1987年   1025篇
  1986年   1076篇
  1985年   1137篇
  1984年   909篇
  1983年   816篇
  1981年   588篇
  1979年   1018篇
  1978年   715篇
  1977年   661篇
  1976年   666篇
  1975年   876篇
  1974年   1052篇
  1973年   1108篇
  1972年   902篇
  1971年   886篇
  1970年   841篇
  1969年   853篇
  1968年   1116篇
  1967年   988篇
  1966年   938篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Range of electric vehicles (EVs) has long been considered a major barrier in acceptance of electric mobility. We examined the nature of how range is experienced in an EV and whether variables from other adaptation contexts, notably stress, have explanatory power for inter‐individual differences in what we term comfortable range. Forty EVs were leased to a sample of users for a 6‐month field study. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of range experiences were performed, including regression analyses to examine the role of stress‐buffering personality traits and coping skills in comfortable range. Users appraised range as a resource to which they could successfully adapt and that satisfied most of their daily mobility needs. However, indicators were found that suggested suboptimal range utilisation. Stress‐buffering personality traits (control beliefs, ambiguity tolerance) and coping skills (subjective range competence, daily range practice) were found to play a substantial role in comfortable range. Hence, it may be possible to overcome perceived range barriers with the assistance of psychological interventions such as information, training, and interface design. Providing drivers with a reliable usable range may be more important than enhancing maximal range in an electric mobility system.  相似文献   
22.
23.
24.
25.
26.
27.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
28.
Our aim was to explore the role of parents in the transmission of stereotypical body size attitudes and awareness of weight loss strategies to preschool children. Participants were 279 3‐year‐old children and their parents, who provided data at baseline and 1 year later. Parents completed self‐report body size attitude and dieting measures. Child weight bias and awareness of weight loss strategies were assessed through interview. Over time, negative associations with large bodies and awareness of weight loss strategies increased. Fathers’ attitudes prospectively predicted boys’ weight bias and awareness of weight loss strategies. Among girls, parental attitudes were less predictive. Findings confirm the importance of fathers in the development of boys’ body attitudes and inform prevention programmes.  相似文献   
29.
We investigated eye‐movements during preschool children's pictorial recall of seen objects. Thirteen 3‐ to 4‐year‐old children completed a perceptual encoding and a pictorial recall task. First, they were exposed to 16 pictorial objects, which were positioned in one of four distinct areas on the computer screen. Subsequently, they had to recall these pictorial objects from memory in order to respond to specific questions about visual details. We found that children spent more time fixating the areas in which the pictorial objects were previously displayed. We conclude that as early as age 3–4 years old, children show specific eye‐movements when they recall pictorial contents of previously seen objects.  相似文献   
30.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号