首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   58711篇
  免费   2243篇
  国内免费   22篇
  2020年   606篇
  2019年   732篇
  2018年   1007篇
  2017年   1077篇
  2016年   1116篇
  2015年   775篇
  2014年   987篇
  2013年   4446篇
  2012年   1766篇
  2011年   1803篇
  2010年   1192篇
  2009年   1093篇
  2008年   1648篇
  2007年   1612篇
  2006年   1462篇
  2005年   1291篇
  2004年   1232篇
  2003年   1184篇
  2002年   1229篇
  2001年   1685篇
  2000年   1646篇
  1999年   1277篇
  1998年   604篇
  1997年   625篇
  1996年   581篇
  1995年   574篇
  1992年   1133篇
  1991年   1013篇
  1990年   1061篇
  1989年   952篇
  1988年   995篇
  1987年   949篇
  1986年   988篇
  1985年   924篇
  1984年   820篇
  1983年   773篇
  1979年   923篇
  1978年   679篇
  1977年   590篇
  1976年   585篇
  1975年   748篇
  1974年   858篇
  1973年   927篇
  1972年   694篇
  1971年   696篇
  1970年   636篇
  1969年   709篇
  1968年   837篇
  1967年   761篇
  1966年   707篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
61.
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers.  相似文献   
62.
Previous research suggests that children gradually understand the mitigating effects of apology on damage to a transgressor's reputation. However, little is known about young children's insights into the central emotional implications of apology. In two studies, children ages 4–9 heard stories about moral transgressions in which the wrongdoers either did or did not apologize. In Study 1, children in the no‐apology condition showed the classic pattern of ‘happy victimizer’ attributions by expecting the wrongdoer to feel good about gains won via transgression. By contrast, in the apology condition, children attributed negative feelings to the transgressor and improved feelings to the victim. In Study 2, these effects were found even when the explicit emotion marker ‘sorry’ was removed from the apology exchange. Thus, young children understand some important emotional functions of apology.  相似文献   
63.
The current study hypothesized that (1) hope would negatively predict burdensomeness, thwarted belongingness, and acquired capability to enact lethal injury; (2) hope would negatively predict suicidal ideation; and (3) the interpersonal suicide risk factors would predict suicidal ideation. Results indicated that hope negatively predicted burdensomeness and thwarted belongingness, but positively predicted acquired capability to enact suicide. Contrary to our second hypothesis, hope did not predict suicidal ideation, but interpersonal risk factors for suicide predicted suicidal ideation. Results are discussed in terms of implications for hope theory and Joiner's (2005) interpersonal risk factors for suicide, and for clinical practice.  相似文献   
64.
65.
66.
67.
68.
69.
70.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号