全文获取类型
收费全文 | 240篇 |
免费 | 0篇 |
专业分类
240篇 |
出版年
2013年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 14篇 |
2009年 | 28篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 3篇 |
2006年 | 1篇 |
2004年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 13篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
1965年 | 6篇 |
1964年 | 8篇 |
1963年 | 10篇 |
1962年 | 7篇 |
1961年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 2篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1955年 | 1篇 |
1953年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有240条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
82.
84.
MELANIE M. DOMENECH RODRÍGUEZ PH.D. MELISSA R. DONOVICK M.S. SUSAN L. CROWLEY PH.D. To read this article in Spanish please see this article's Supporting Information on Wiley InterScience . 《Family process》2009,48(2):195-210
Current literature presents four primary parenting styles: authoritative, authoritarian, permissive, and neglectful. These styles provide an important shortcut for a constellation of parenting behaviors that have been characterized as consisting of warmth, demandingness, and autonomy granting. Empirically, only warmth and demandingness are typically measured. Research reporting on parenting styles in Latino samples has been equivocal leading to questions about conceptualization and measurement of parenting styles in this ethnic/cultural group. This lack of consensus may result from the chasm between concepts (e.g., authoritarian parenting) and observable parenting behaviors (e.g., warmth) in this ethnic group. The present research aimed to examine parenting styles and dimensions in a sample of Latino parents using the two usual dimensions (warmth, demandingness) and adding autonomy granting. Traditional parenting styles categories were examined, as well as additional categorizations that resulted from adding autonomy granting. Fifty first-generation Latino parents and their child (aged 4–9) participated. Parent–child interactions were coded with the Parenting Style Observation Rating Scale (P-SOS). In this sample, the four traditional parenting categories did not capture Latino families well. The combination of characteristics resulted in eight possible parenting styles. Our data showed the majority (61%) of Latino parents as \"protective parents.\" Further, while mothers and fathers were similar in their parenting styles, expectations were different for male and female children. The additional dimensions and implications are discussed. The importance of considering the cultural context in understanding parenting in Latino families is emphasized, along with directions for future research. 相似文献
85.
86.
87.
To read this article in Spanish, please see this article's Supporting Information on Wiley InterScience (http://interscience.wiley.com/journal/famp). To read this article's abstract in both Spanish and Mandarin Chinese, please visit the article's full‐text page on Wiley InterScience ( http://interscience.wiley.com/journal/famp ). The aim of the study is to explore the process of microtransitions in families with adolescent children. Original methodological procedures were designed in order to have families as the objects of study and to analyze data with particular attention to the family process of change. A family interview focused on the adolescent and family change was conducted with 12 families having an adolescent child. As indicators of change, we used coordination and oscillation. Our results highlight different patterns of family interactions, illustrating various ways through which families deal with change. Conclusive remarks focus on the theoretical relevance of the study, the method and the implications for family practice and policy. 相似文献
88.
Inhibitory control has been suggested as a key predictive measure of problem-solving skills in human and nonhuman animals. However, there has yet to be a direct comparison of the inhibitory skills of the nonhuman apes and their development in human children. We compared the inhibitory skills of all great ape species, including 3–5-year-old children in a detour-reaching task, which required subjects to avoid reaching directly for food and instead use an indirect reaching method to successfully obtain the food. We tested 22 chimpanzees, 18 bonobos, 18 orangutans, 6 gorillas and 42 children. Our sample included chimpanzees, bonobos and orangutans housed in zoos (N = 27) and others housed in sanctuaries in their native habitats (N = 37). Overall, orangutans were the most skilful apes, including human children. As expected older children outperformed younger children. Sanctuary chimpanzees and bonobos outperformed their zoo counterparts whereas there was no difference between the two orangutan samples. Most zoo chimpanzees and bonobos failed to solve the original task, but improved their performance with additional training, although the training method determined to a considerable extent the level of success that the apes achieved in a transfer phase. In general, the performance of the older children was far from perfect and comparable to some of the nonhuman apes tested. 相似文献
89.
JAMES McHALE PH.D. ELISABETH FIVAZ‐DEPEURSINGE PH.D. SUSAN DICKSTEIN PH.D. JANET ROBERTSON B.A. MATTHEW DALEY B.A. 《Family process》2008,47(4):445-463
Infants appear to be active participants in complex interactional sequences with their parents far earlier than previously theorized. In this report, we document the capacity of 3‐month‐old infants to share attention with two partners (mothers and fathers) simultaneously, and trace links between this capacity and early family group‐level dynamics. During comprehensive evaluations of the family's emergent coparenting alliance completed in 113 homes, we charted infants' eye gaze patterns during two different mother‐father‐infant assessment paradigms. Triangular capacities (operationalized as the frequency of rapid multishift gaze transitions between parents during interactions) were stable across interaction context. Infants exhibiting more advanced triangular capacities belonged to families showing evidence of better coparental adjustment. Theoretical and practice implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
90.
MO YEE LEE GILBERT J. GREENE PH.D. LISW IMFT KAI SHYANG HSU PH.D. † Y SOLOVEY MSW LISW ‡ DAVID GROVE MSW LISW § J. SCOTT FRASER PH.D. ¶ PHIL WASHBURN R.N. BARBARA TEATER PH.D. 《Family process》2009,48(3):395-416
Community mental health agencies are consistently challenged to provide realistic and effective home-based family-centered treatment that meets local needs and can realistically fit within available budget and resource capabilities. Integrated Family and Systems Treatment (I-FAST) is developed based on existing evidence-based approaches for working with at-risk children, adolescents, and families and a strengths perspective. I-FAST identified 3 evidence-based, core treatment components and integrated them into a coherent treatment protocol; this is done in a way that builds on and is integrated with mental health agencies' existing expertise in home-based treatment. This is an intervention development study in which we conducted an initial feasibility trial of I-FAST for treating families with children at risk of out-of-home placement. The outcomes of the study provide initial empirical evidence that supports the effectiveness of I-FAST. Findings indicate that there were significant improvements in child behavior, significant increases in parental competency, and significant increases in the level of cohesion and adaptability in these families. All observed changes were significant from pre- to posttreatment with the families able to maintain these positive changes at 6-month follow-up. A more rigorous and robust research design, however, will be needed to establish definitive evidence of the effectiveness of I-FAST. 相似文献