全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1693篇 |
免费 | 72篇 |
专业分类
1765篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 24篇 |
2022年 | 36篇 |
2021年 | 57篇 |
2020年 | 47篇 |
2019年 | 54篇 |
2018年 | 105篇 |
2017年 | 85篇 |
2016年 | 73篇 |
2015年 | 52篇 |
2014年 | 61篇 |
2013年 | 210篇 |
2012年 | 129篇 |
2011年 | 125篇 |
2010年 | 98篇 |
2009年 | 43篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 77篇 |
2006年 | 75篇 |
2005年 | 41篇 |
2004年 | 36篇 |
2003年 | 28篇 |
2002年 | 28篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 19篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1963年 | 3篇 |
1962年 | 2篇 |
1961年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1765条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
We present two experiments in which we measured lexical decision latencies and errors to words with few or many orthographic neighbors (ie., Coltheart's N). The main goal of the study was to examine whether or not the neighborhood size effect in a lexical decision task could be affected by the exposure duration of the stimulus item (unlimited vs. limited time exposure, 150 msec plus a backward mask) and the type of decision involved in the task (yes/no vs. go/no-go lexical decision tasks). In the yes/no task, the results showed a facilitative neighborhood size effect for low frequency that did not interact with exposure duration (Experiment 1). In contrast, in the go/no-go task (in this task, participants are instructed to respond as quickly as they can when a word is presented and not to respond if a nonword is presented), the neighborhood size effect for low-frequency words (and for nonwords) was greater under limited viewing time (Experiment 2). In addition, the word frequency effect was greater in the go/no-go task than in the yes/no task, replicating Hino and Lupker (1998, 2000). The results were interpreted in terms of the interaction of decision and lexical factors in visual-word recognition. 相似文献
82.
The process of learning the structure of novel objects involves the selective use of information available in the distal stimulus. By allowing participants to explore the object within a limited field of view, we were able to examine more rigorously what regions of the object are actually selected in the learning process. Participants explored objects either by moving a circular aperture over a stationary novel object (the aperture-movement condition), or by moving the object behind a stationary aperture (the object-movement condition). Given the differences in how the spatial layout of object parts is revealed in the two study conditions, we expected that exploration would be more systematic in the aperture-movement condition than it would be in the object-movement condition, and would lead to better object recognition. We show evidence that in the aperture-movement condition exploration patterns were more related to the structure of the object and, as a consequence, the aperture-movement condition resulted in more accurate recognition in a later old--new discrimination test. 相似文献
83.
Koronczai B Urbán R Kökönyei G Paksi B Papp K Kun B Arnold P Kállai J Demetrovics Z 《Cyberpsychology, behavior and social networking》2011,14(11):657-664
As the Internet became widely used, problems associated with its excessive use became increasingly apparent. Although for the assessment of these problems several models and related questionnaires have been elaborated, there has been little effort made to confirm them. The aim of the present study was to test the three-factor model of the previously created Problematic Internet Use Questionnaire (PIUQ) by data collection methods formerly not applied (off-line group and face-to-face settings), on the one hand, and by testing on different age groups (adolescent and adult representative samples), on the other hand. Data were collected from 438 high-school students (44.5 percent boys; mean age: 16.0 years; standard deviation=0.7 years) and also from 963 adults (49.9 percent males; mean age: 33.6 years; standard deviation=11.8 years). We applied confirmatory factor analysis to confirm the measurement model of problematic Internet use. The results of the analyses carried out inevitably support the original three-factor model over the possible one-factor solution. Using latent profile analysis, we identified 11 percent of adults and 18 percent of adolescent users characterized by problematic use. Based on exploratory factor analysis, we also suggest a short form of the PIUQ consisting of nine items. Both the original 18-item version of PIUQ and its short 9-item form have satisfactory reliability and validity characteristics, and thus, they are suitable for the assessment of problematic Internet use in future studies. 相似文献
84.
A scale to measure attitudes towards seeking psychological help was developed (Beliefs About Psychological Services [BAPS]) and evaluated. This scale was based on items gathered from students, colleagues, and the authors when asked about common positive and negative attitudes toward psychologists and their services. Items were also based on the Fischer and Turner (1970) Attitudes Toward Seeking Professional Psychological Services (ATSPPH) measure. Three studies are reported describing the construction and evaluation of the psychometric properties of the BAPS. The BAPS is an 18-item measure consisting of three subscales: Intent, Stigma Tolerance, and Expertness. Support was found for the reliability and validity of the measure. 相似文献
85.
Iris Blandón‐Gitlin Kathy Pezdek D. Stephen Lindsay Lisa Hagen 《Applied cognitive psychology》2009,23(7):901-917
Worldwide, the criteria‐based content analysis (CBCA) is probably the most widely used veracity assessment technique for discriminating between accounts of true and fabricated events. In this study, two experiments examined the effectiveness of the CBCA for discriminating between accounts of true events and suggested events believed to be true. In Experiment 1, CBCA‐trained judges evaluated participants' accounts of true and suggestively planted childhood events. In Experiment 2, judges analysed accounts of recent events that were experimentally manipulated to be a (a) true experience, (b) false experience believed to be true and (c) deliberately fabricated experience. In both experiments CBCA scores were significantly higher for accounts of true events than suggested events. However, this difference was not significant for participants classified as experiencing ‘full’ memories for the suggested event. Self‐report memory measures supported the findings of the CBCA analyses. Taken together these results suggest that the CBCA discriminative power is greatly constrained. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
86.
87.
Dan López de Sa 《Synthese》2009,170(1):1-5
In recent years, some people have held that a radical relativist position is defensible in some philosophically interesting
cases, including future contingents, predicates of personal taste, evaluative predicates in general, epistemic modals, and
knowledge attributions. The position is frequently characterized as denying that utterance-truth is absolute. I argue that
this characterization is inappropriate, as it requires a metaphysical substantive contention with which moderate views as
such need not be committed. Before this, I also offer a more basic, admittedly less exciting alternative characterization
of the position, in terms of departing from the Kaplan–Lewis–Stalnaker two-dimensional framework. 相似文献
88.
The ability to discriminate between larger and smaller quantities has been demonstrated in several mammalian and avian species
suggesting the possibility of evolutionary conservation of this characteristic. Preference for the larger of two groups has
also been shown in fish species, although this ability has rarely been systematically studied in lower order vertebrates,
and thus the mechanisms of such ability are not understood. Here, we exploit the tendency of angelfish to seek protection
in an unfamiliar environment by joining a group of conspecifics, a behaviour called shoaling. Test fish were given a simultaneous
choice between shoals varying both in terms of numerical ratios and absolute numbers of fish. Our results provide evidence
for quantity discrimination in angelfish. In general, experimental subjects chose the larger of two shoals. Furthermore, in
agreement with Weber’s law, which holds that discrimination between two quantities depends on their ratio, the discrimination
between shoals of different quantities of fish was more difficult when the shoal sizes became more similar. The limit of discrimination
ratio was found to be below 2:1. Briefly, angelfish are able to discriminate between different quantities of conspecifics
subject to a ratio limit, a finding that implies a fitness component in this behaviour similar to what has been demonstrated
in higher order vertebrates. 相似文献
89.
Gómez-Lázaro E Arregi A Beitia G Vegas O Azpiroz A Garmendia L 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2011,14(5):537-548
This study aimed to analyze different behavioral profiles in response to chronic social defeat using the sensorial contact model. We hypothesized that a passive profile, unlike an active one, would be associated with behavioral and physiological characteristics related to depression. Six-week-old OF1 male mice were subjected to defeat for 21 consecutive days. A combination of cluster and discriminant analyses of the behavior exhibited during confrontation on Day 21 established two behavioral profiles: active (n?=?22) and passive (n?=?34). Passive mice, with a high level of immobility and low non-social exploration, had higher plasma corticosterone concentrations than active mice after 21 days of defeat. Three days after the last defeat, passive mice had lower corticosterone levels than manipulated-control mice (n?=?11). Higher levels of interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) in the spleen and lower hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels were observed in passive mice in comparison with those in active mice and the manipulated controls. The only differences observed in active mice in relation to the manipulated control were higher plasma corticosterone (Day 21) and TNF-α levels. The results show that different behavioral profiles in response to chronic defeat are associated with different physiological profiles, and that the passive profile presents physiological characteristics previously associated with depression. 相似文献
90.
This paper describes the development of a self-report scale for adolescents to assess positive youth development values. It presents a substantial number of psychometric results performed on a sample of 2,400 adolescents (1,068 boys and 1,332 girls) from 12 to 17 years ( M = 14.73, SD = 1.25), who were studying secondary education in Western Andalusia. The results provide evidence of the psychometric quality of items, cross-validity of a structure of eight first-order factors and three second-order factors, obtained through exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, and adequate reliability. This study concludes that the scale presents adequate evidence of validity and reliability that allows assessment, especially in school contexts, of a broad set of values of particular relevance to positive youth development. 相似文献