全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2885篇 |
免费 | 162篇 |
专业分类
3047篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 36篇 |
2022年 | 57篇 |
2021年 | 78篇 |
2020年 | 109篇 |
2019年 | 89篇 |
2018年 | 166篇 |
2017年 | 168篇 |
2016年 | 139篇 |
2015年 | 79篇 |
2014年 | 103篇 |
2013年 | 381篇 |
2012年 | 220篇 |
2011年 | 223篇 |
2010年 | 136篇 |
2009年 | 69篇 |
2008年 | 187篇 |
2007年 | 140篇 |
2006年 | 120篇 |
2005年 | 67篇 |
2004年 | 59篇 |
2003年 | 69篇 |
2002年 | 44篇 |
2001年 | 26篇 |
2000年 | 35篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 17篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
1965年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有3047条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
The possibility that rats can navigate in the Morris water maze by reducing the difference between the memorized platform scene and the current sensory input was tested in nine blind rats. A computerized videosystem monitored the rats' movements in the pool and converted the rat-target distance into tones the frequency of which increased in 64 equal steps from 120 Hz at 128 cm to 7680 Hz at 0 cm. During 15 days of training to find a fixed platform position from different starting points (12 trials per day) average escape latencies decreased from 39.0 to 25.4 s. The performance significantly deteriorated when the acoustic distance signalization was omitted and/or when the target position was changed form trial to trial. It is concluded that blind rats solved the task by simultaneously employing search strategy based on position responses, mapping using acoustic background beacons, and distance reduction navigation. It is argued that the various strategies are additive and that their relative significance depends of the conditions of the experiment. 相似文献
53.
In this article, a leadership model is presented, with which to investigate the relationship of trait emotional intelligence (trait EI), leadership self-efficacy and leader's task self-efficacy with collective task efficacy and group performance. The sample was made up of 217 undergraduate students, randomly assigned to work teams of 1 leader and 2 followers that were requested to perform a production task. An adapted version of the Schutte Self-Report Inventory (SSRI; Schutte et al., 1998) was used to measure trait EI. Structural equation modeling was used to test the hypothesized relationships. Results indicated that task self-efficacy was a mediator between leadership self-efficacy and collective task efficacy; the latter, in turn, was the best predictor of group performance. No significant relationship was found between trait EI and collective task efficacy although, unexpectedly, trait EI was positively associated with leadership self-efficacy. Implications of the results are discussed. 相似文献
54.
Moreno Jiménez B Rodríguez Muñioz A Martínez Gamarra M Gálvez Herrer M 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2007,10(2):449-457
The aim of the present study was to validate a reduced Spanish version of the Negative Acts Questionnaire (NAQ; Einarsen & Raknes, 1997). This instrument, which has been widely used in various studies, was developed to measure workplace bullying. Two samples, the first comprising 352 employees from 11 organizations, and the second comprising victims of bullying who were recruited from 15 Spanish associations against bullying, participated in the study. Exploratory factor analysis conducted with the data from the first sample revealed a two-factor solution that accounted for 63.3% of the total variance. The data from the second sample were used for confirmatory factor analyses to compare three structure models of the NAQ (one factor, two independent factors, and two correlated factors). The results indicate that the correlated two-factor model provided the best fit to the data (chi2/df = 2.1, CFI = .93, GFI = .95, RMR = .04, RMSEA = .06, AIC = 215.4). Reliability analysis showed that this 14-item Spanish version had high internal consistency. Significant correlations between the NAQ and its dimensions and diverse health and perceived stress scales were found, which provided evidence of construct validity. Taken conjointly, the results of this study support the use of the Spanish version of the reduced NAQ in future research. 相似文献
55.
Kristjánsson A 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2007,48(2):97-113
This paper presents a review and summary of experimental findings on the role of attention in the preparation of saccadic eye movements. The focus is on experiments where performance of prosaccades (saccades towards a suddenly appearing item) and antisaccades (saccades of equal amplitude in the direction opposite to where the target moved) is compared. Evidence suggests that these two opposite responses to the same stimulus event entail competition between neural pathways that generate reflexive movements to the target and neural mechanisms involved in inhibiting the reflex and generating a voluntary gaze shift in the opposite direction to the target appearance. Evidence for such a competition account is discussed in light of a large amount of experimental findings and the overall picture clearly indicates that this competition account has great explanatory power when data on saccadic reaction times and error rates are compared for the two types of saccade. The role of attention is also discussed in particular in light of the finding that the withdrawal of attention by a secondary task 200 to 500 ms before the saccade target appears, leads to speeded antisaccades (without a similar increase in error rates), showing that the results do not simply reflect a speed-accuracy trade-off. This result indicates that the tendency for "reflexive" prosaccades is diminished when attention is engaged in a different task. Furthermore, experiments are discussed that show that as the tendency for a reflexive prosaccade is weakened, antisaccades are speeded up, further supporting the competition account of pro- and antisaccade generation. In the light of evidence from neurophysiology of monkeys and humans, a tentative model of pro- and antisaccade generation is proposed. 相似文献
56.
Attending, ignoring, and repetition: on the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return 总被引:8,自引:0,他引:8
A series of spatial localization experiments is reported that addresses the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return. The results of Experiment 1 demonstrate that slowed responses to repeated location stimuli can be obscured by repetition priming effects involving stimulus dimensions other than spatial location. The results of Experiments 2, 3A, and 3B demonstrate that these repetition priming effects may occur only when participants are required to respond to the prime display. Together, these results suggest that differences between attended and ignored repetition effects in selective attention studies of spatial localization do not provide a basis for distinguishing between spatial negative priming and inhibition of return. 相似文献
57.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
58.
Psychometrika - Using the well-known strategy in which parameters are linked to the sampling distribution via an identification analysis, we offer an interpretation of the item parameters in the... 相似文献
59.
Philosophia - Critical statements, if true, bear ontological commitments to fictional entities. A well-known version of fictionalism about fictional characters tries to eliminate these ontological... 相似文献
60.
dos Santos Bibiana Ramos Sarriera Jorge Castellá Bedin Lívia Maria 《Applied research in quality of life》2019,14(3):819-835
Applied Research in Quality of Life - This study aims associating children’s satisfaction to their interpersonal relationships within their main contexts of interaction (family, friends and... 相似文献