全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1705篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
专业分类
1722篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 42篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 92篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 79篇 |
2016年 | 89篇 |
2015年 | 73篇 |
2014年 | 77篇 |
2013年 | 329篇 |
2012年 | 52篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 50篇 |
2009年 | 63篇 |
2008年 | 37篇 |
2007年 | 21篇 |
2006年 | 44篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 14篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 46篇 |
1995年 | 34篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 41篇 |
1990年 | 35篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 21篇 |
1977年 | 26篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1965年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1722条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Prof. Dr. Zekiye Demir A. Ömer Toprak PhD in Economics 《Teaching Theology & Religion》2023,26(1):29-43
This paper studied a new dimension of international students, who are citizens of another country but came back to Turkey for religious education where their parents or grandparents are citizens. Did a five-year religious education process based on the main sources of Islam lead to a change in these students' religious attitudes and behaviors, and if yes to what extent did these changes occur? A panel survey is conducted on these students in pre-education and post-education periods to answer this question. Without ignoring the effect of the social environment outside of education on religious attitudes, we found that students exhibit a more tolerant, nonstrict attitude toward both their coreligionists and those who have negative attitudes toward their religion. Additionally, in parallel with the deepening of religious knowledge, a questioning and critical perspective was formed with a decrease in superstitions. We found that the attitude change in female students was generally higher than that of male students. 相似文献
972.
973.
Thematic analysis methods, including the reflexive approach we have developed, are widely used in counselling and psychotherapy research, as are other approaches that seek to develop ‘patterns’ (themes, categories) across cases. Without a thorough grounding in the conceptual foundations of a wide variety of across‐case analytic approaches, and qualitative research more broadly—something rarely offered in counselling training—it can be difficult to understand how these differ, where they overlap, and which might be appropriate for a particular research project. Our aim in this paper is to support researchers in counselling and psychotherapy to select an appropriate across‐case approach for their research, and to justify their choice, by discussing conceptual and procedural differences and similarities between reflexive thematic analysis (TA) and four other across‐case approaches. Three of these are also widely used in counselling and psychotherapy research—qualitative content analysis, interpretative phenomenological analysis and grounded theory. The fourth—discourse analysis—is less widely used but importantly exemplifies the critical qualitative research tradition. We contextualise our comparative approach by highlighting the diversity within TA. TA is best thought of as a spectrum of methods—from types that prioritise coding accuracy and reliability to reflexive approaches like ours that emphasise the inescapable subjectivity of data interpretation. Although reflexive TA provides the point of comparison for our discussion of other across‐case approaches, our aim is not to promote reflexive TA as ‘best’. Rather, we encourage the knowing selection and use of analytic methods and methodologies in counselling and psychotherapy research. 相似文献
974.
Bruce G. Taylor PhD Elizabeth A. Mumford PhD Nnenna Okeke PhD Emily Rothman ScD 《Aggressive behavior》2020,46(1):25-36
Research has shown that neighborhoods play a role in the etiology of violence. However, few adolescent relationship aggression (ARA) studies have objective measures of violent neighborhoods. Drawing on a nationally representative sample of youth, this study examines the association between ARA and local levels of violent crime (measured using geocoded Uniform Crime Report data from each of the youths’ residential neighborhoods). Study analyses are based on survey data from 723 youth (ages 10–18) in current or recent dating relationships (351 males and 372 females) in the Survey on Teen Relationships and Intimate Violence (STRiV), a national representative household panel survey exploring interpersonal violence and related aggression among adolescents. About 19% of the sample reported ARA victimization in their most recent dating relationship (ARA perpetration was 17%). Neighborhood violent crime in the study (males living in 86.9 and females 99.8) was slightly lower than the national average of 100. With a broad national sample, 40% non-Whites, hypotheses guided by theories of neighborhood influence were tested. The study did not find an association between neighborhood violent crime and ARA victimization and perpetration, controlling for key demographic factors. The results, for a broad range of high- and low-crime neighborhoods, suggest that neighborhood violence does not seem to affect individual rates of ARA. The results suggest the ARA victimization and perpetration are perhaps ubiquitous and found both in low and high violent crime neighborhoods, suggesting that addressing local violent crime rates alone does not seem to be a path to also reducing ARA. 相似文献
975.
The general aggression model (GAM) has suggested that the interaction between person factors (e.g., personality variables) and situation factors (e.g., playing violent video games [VVGs]) can increase individuals’ aggressive behaviors through their cognition (e.g., hostile attributions), affect (e.g., negative affect), and/or arousal. The present study employed a modified competitive reaction time task to test the effects of shyness, violent (vs. nonviolent) gameplay, and shyness on individuals’ positive–negative affect, hostile attributions, and aggressive behaviors. In addition, the present study also employed structural equation modeling (SEM) to test the mediation (by cognition and affect) and moderation (by shyness). Results showed that playing a VVG increased aggressive behaviors, negative affect, and hostile attributions primarily among shy participants. In addition, the results of SEM also revealed that this moderating role was mediated by negative affect and hostile attributions. The present study supported GAM and showed that individuals’ aggressive behaviors are differentially susceptible to VVGs, depending on their level of shyness in a “for bad and for worse” manner. 相似文献
976.
977.
978.
979.
980.