全文获取类型
收费全文 | 21654篇 |
免费 | 77篇 |
专业分类
21731篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 26篇 |
2018年 | 3510篇 |
2017年 | 2840篇 |
2016年 | 2275篇 |
2015年 | 220篇 |
2014年 | 111篇 |
2013年 | 208篇 |
2012年 | 615篇 |
2011年 | 2430篇 |
2010年 | 2535篇 |
2009年 | 1500篇 |
2008年 | 1730篇 |
2007年 | 2205篇 |
2006年 | 63篇 |
2005年 | 235篇 |
2004年 | 199篇 |
2003年 | 150篇 |
2002年 | 104篇 |
2001年 | 55篇 |
2000年 | 72篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 35篇 |
1997年 | 34篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 21篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 23篇 |
1990年 | 28篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 23篇 |
1984年 | 27篇 |
1983年 | 15篇 |
1982年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
1965年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Recent evidence indicates that central directional stimuli, such as eyes and arrows, trigger rapid, reflexive shifts of spatial attention. A study by Fischer, Castel, Dodd, and Pratt (2003) suggested that a similar effect might also apply to central numbers, as if a digit's meaning causes attention to be oriented to its relative position on a left-to-right mental number line. However, unlike central eyes and arrows, the orienting effect for central digits emerges slowly, suggesting that top-down endogenous processes may be mediating this effect. Here, we report a series of three experiments that strongly support this hypothesis. Experiment 1 replicated Fischer et al.'s left-to-right number line effect. Experiment 2 showed that this effect could be completely reversed by merely asking participants to imagine a number line running from right to left. Experiment 3 showed that a left-to-right number line effect could be abolished by presenting targets above and below central fixation, as well as to the left and right of center. Experiment 3 also showed that other mental sets, such as imagining a clock, result in attention's being oriented in accordance with where the central digits are represented on a clock face. Together, these data indicate that the spatial representations and attentional orienting related to the perception of digits are both fragile and flexible and depend critically on the top-down spatial mental sets adopted by individuals. 相似文献
62.
63.
Marcia Wright Kassner 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》1981,18(3):340-355
This study examined the significance of work preference variables, family preference variables, socialization influences, and needs for achievement and affiliation in predicting preferred traditional versus egalitarian marriages among university students. Results showed that, overall, male university students preferred traditional marriages while female university students preferred egalitarian marriages. Sex also interacted significantly with the following variables: desired family task involvement, desired job task involvement, and mother's family involvement. The data suggest that a perspective encompassing a wide variety of variables (including work, family, and socialization considerations) may be useful for conceptualizing individuals' preferences for traditional or egalitarian marriages. Implications of the results for management and career counseling are discussed. 相似文献
64.
The present study investigated whether auditory temporal processing deficits are related to the presence and/or the severity of periventricular brain injury and the reading difficulties experienced by extremely low birthweight (ELBW: birthweight <1000 g) children. Results indicate that ELBW children with mild or severe brain lesions obtained significantly lower scores on a test requiring auditory temporal order judgments than ELBW children without periventricular brain injury or children who were full-term. Structural equation modeling indicated that a model in which auditory temporal processing deficits predicted speech sound discrimination and phonological processing ability provided a better fit for the data than did a second model, which hypothesized that auditory temporal processing deficits are associated with poor reading abilities through a working memory deficit. These findings suggest that an impairment in auditory temporal processing may contribute to the reading difficulties experienced by ELBW children. 相似文献
65.
Warren R. Stanton 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2002,21(2):176-198
Stage theories are commonly used to represent the structure of cognitive processes in the domains of cognitive processing
and cognitive development. Hierarchical unidimensional theories consisting of a single sequence of stages through which development
proceeds in one direction only have predominated. However, a review of stage theories revealed that a two-dimensional, interactive
(multidimensional) structure is warranted. Particularly in the area of cognitive development, results of research have supported
subdivision of the stages of prominent unidimensional theories. Following this trend a small number of theorists have developed
multidimensional theories, most of which consist of a sequence of stages in which each stage contains a number of subsidiary
stages or levels. The emergence of a similar trend in other domains is examined, and the prospects of using an existing multidimensional
framework to enable future comparisons and integration of different stage theories is explored. 相似文献
66.
Online counselling: learning from writing therapy 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Jeannie Wright 《British Journal of Guidance & Counselling》2002,30(3):285-298
Online counselling is new and still contentious, with little UK-based research available to practitioners, providers or potential clients. This article aims to extend an earlier review of some of the research into writing therapy \[Wright &; Chung (2001) Mastery or mystery? Therapeutic writing: a review of the literature, British Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 29(3), 277-291] and to indicate how it could be applied to online counselling. It also refers to some of the literature on online counselling, which, together with the writing therapy research, informed the decision to offer an online service to staff in a university setting. 相似文献
67.
68.
Jonathan James McCreadie Lillie 《Sexuality & culture》2002,6(2):25-48
This article presents theoretical considerations based on cultural analysis approaches to studying pornography and sexuality
as a means of starting to suggest a new agenda for cyberporn research. By bringing to the forefront concepts of how subjectivity
and sexuality are produced within the computer/Internet apparatus, I hope to diversify the focus in cyberporn research away
from social science approaches and pre-Foucaultian assumptions of the subject which obscure understandings of new media and
cyberporn use. Through a summary of visual culture studies and reception studies of pornography, I argue that cyberporn must
be understood as contingent within the encoding and decoding processes and discourses of sexuality (Foucault) in which it
is produced and consumed. My focus here is the home office/terminal as the site of reception/cyberporn use. While there is
potential for a great variety of cultural analytic approaches to the study of cyberporn and how new media use influences sexuality,
I end with specific suggestions for researching cyberporn reception in the home.
The computer’s allure is more than utilitarian or aesthetic; it is erotic. Instead of a refreshing play with surfaces, as
with toys or amusements, our affair with information machines announces a symbiotic relationship and ultimately a mental marriage
to technology. Rightly perceived, the atmosphere of cyberspace carries the scent that once surrounded Wisdom. The world rendered
as pure information not only fascinates our eyes and minds, but also captures our hearts. We feel augmented and empowered.
Our hearts beat in the machines. This is Eros. (Michael Heim, The Metaphysics of Virtual Reality) 相似文献
69.
Sara‐Jayne Williams Daniel B. Wright Norman H. Freeman 《Applied cognitive psychology》2002,16(6):651-664
Children are generally more susceptible than adults to suggestive interview techniques. Children's memories of an event can be altered and added to by presenting post‐event information (PEI). What is not known is whether embedding silence about a particular scene within the PEI makes that scene less likely to be reported. Children aged 5–6 years made cakes with an agent ‘Mrs Flour’. The following day they received PEI in which a target scene from the original event was omitted, resulting in children reporting the target scene significantly less often than did controls (control= 57% and omit= 23% correct responses). There was direct evidence from the children's language that the omission led to a detriment in memory for the original scene itself. Allowing children to draw during the interview did not reduce the effect. Implications are discussed in terms of child victims and witnesses particularly regarding child sexual abuse. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
70.