全文获取类型
收费全文 | 730篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
741篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 24篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 25篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 66篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 42篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1958年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有741条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
172.
The purpose of the present article was to demonstrate a systemic treatment of depression for a 70 year-old woman. In this treatment, other family members were unavailable, but a family therapy systems perspective was used throughout. Results of the demonstration ABAB single case design and two year followup were suggestive of treatment effectiveness in decreasing depression. The need for additional replication of the study was noted. 相似文献
173.
174.
J J Furedy O Algan A Vincent S Demirgoren S Pogun 《Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science》1999,34(4):219-226
In a two-day, two-session experiment where smokers male and female college-student subjects worked on a cognitive verbal task during either the first or second day, and on a cognitive spatial task on the second or first day, smoking was manipulated as an acute independent variable by requiring 10+ hours of pre-experimental abstention, and providing a cigarette during the 15-minute rest period between the two sessions. Non-smoker female and male subjects underwent the same experiment, and hence served as controls for the effects of this acute-smoking manipulation. Overall adaptation (decreased arousal) to the experiment was manifested in a significant increase in skin resistance level (SRL) in all subjects, but when this adaptation effect was statistically controlled, there was a significant smokers by sex interaction during the verbal task only, such that SRL was increased by the cigarette in males, but decreased in females. In contrast, the same analysis indicated only a marked increase in heart-rate (HR) due to smoking, which was unaffected either by sex or by whether the task was the verbal or the (easier) spatial one. We interpret the SRL results as reflecting a sex difference in the direction of transient psychological arousal, and discuss it in relation to evidence in the literature based on self reports, and to evidence (based on HR in this study and on blood pressure in other studies) on physiological (cardiovascular) arousal. Key Words: Electrodermal activity, heart-rate, psychological vs. physiological, verbal and spatial cognitive tasks. 相似文献
175.
Ron Acierno Michel Hersen Geoffrey Tremont Vincent B. Van Hasselt Robert Kabacoff 《Journal of Clinical Geropsychology》1999,5(4):245-263
The DSM-IV does not subclassify patients with depression on the basis of anxiety level. Hence a significant confound may exist in all outcome studies that employ DSM definitions of depression. To establish that objectively identifiable anxious and nonanxious subtypes of depression do indeed exist, a psychophysiological assessment battery was used with 114 treatment-seeking older adults. Dichotomous criterion categorization as either Nonanxious Depressed or Anxious Depressed was based on (a) DSM-III-R/DSM-IV diagnosis, and (b) standardized questionnaires of psychopathology. Multivariate analyses revealed no differences between groups when DSM criteria were used to classify participants. However, identical analyses using phenomenological diagnostic criteria indicated that anxious and nonanxious depressed participants differed in their psychophysiological response to negative imagery. Although anxious and nonanxious depressed participants evince different psychophysiological response patterns, these differences unfortunately are obscured by the DSM. Consequently, a phenomenological classification system may be more appropriate with affective disorders. 相似文献
176.
Two experiments yielded further evidence for the black sheep effect (Marques, Yzerbyt and by ens, 1988). In the first experiment, 66 subjects were presented with two good or two poor speeches, one supposedly made by an ingroup member and the other supposedly made by an outgroup member. In the second experiment, 37 subjects were presented with one good and one poor speech supposedly made either by two ingroup members or by two outgroup members. The black sheep effect was predicted and found in both experiments: subjects over-evaluated likeable ingroup members and under-evaluated unlikeable ingroup members as compared to equally likeable and unlikeable outgroup members. Collapsing the data of the two experiments suggests that social comparison may be performed, in purely symbolic terms, against a cognitive standard of positivity rather than an outgroup present in the judgmental situation. The emergence of the predicted effect when strongly individualized information was presented in inter- as well as in intra-group situations supports the robustness of the black sheep effect. 相似文献
177.
Jensen?T.?MeccaEmail author Carter?Gibson Vincent?Giorgini Kelsey?E.?Medeiros Michael?D.?Mumford Shane?Connelly 《Science and engineering ethics》2015,21(4):843-855
The increasing interconnectedness of academic research and external industry has left research vulnerable to conflicts of interest. These conflicts have the potential to undermine the integrity of scientific research as well as to threaten public trust in scientific findings. The present effort sought to identify themes in the perspectives of faculty researchers regarding conflicts of interest. Think-aloud interview responses were qualitatively analyzed in an effort to provide insights with regard to appropriate ways to address the threat of conflicts of interest in research. Themes in participant responses included disclosure of conflicts of interest, self-removal from situations where conflict exists, accommodation of conflict, denial of the existence of conflict, and recognition of complexity of situations involving conflicts of interest. Moral disengagement operations are suggested to explain the appearance of each identified theme. In addition, suggestions for best practices regarding addressing conflicts of interest given these themes in faculty perspectives are provided. 相似文献
178.
179.
Chang-Jiang Dong Nicholas V. Swindale Pierre Zakarauskas Vincent Hayward Max S. Cynader 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2000,62(5):1099-1111
In this paper, the auditory motion aftereffect (aMAE) was studied, using real moving sound as both the adapting and the test stimulus. The sound was generated by a loudspeaker mounted on a robot arm that was able to move quietly in three-dimensional space. A total of 7 subjects with normal hearing were tested in three experiments. The results from Experiment 1 showed a robust and reliable negative aMAE in all the subjects. After listening to a sound source moving repeatedly to the right, a stationary sound source was perceived to move to the left. The magnitude of the aMAE tended to increase with adapting velocity up to the highest velocity tested (20°/sec). The aftereffect was largest when the adapting and the test stimuli had similar spatial location and frequency content. Offsetting the locations of the adapting and the test stimuli by 20° reduced the size of the effect by about 50%. A similar decline occurred when the frequency of the adapting and the test stimuli differed by one octave. Our results suggest that the human auditory system possesses specialized mechanisms for detecting auditory motion in the spatial domain. 相似文献
180.