全文获取类型
收费全文 | 727篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
738篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 25篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 66篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 32篇 |
2008年 | 23篇 |
2007年 | 28篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 25篇 |
2002年 | 14篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 10篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1958年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有738条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
251.
Sociometric measures were used to predict peer helpfulness of youth in a residential treatment center. Seventeen female adolescents with conduct disorders were asked to choose three peers in their group who were most popular, similar, well-liked, and competent. These sociometric measures were correlated with the frequency in which each peer was nominated as most and least helpful in several problem scenarios representing actual situations encountered in residential settings. A multiple linear regression analysis indicated that “popularity” was the best predictor of peer helpfulness across the problem scenarios. Discussion focused on the impact of popularity on treatment strategies for youth in residential placement. 相似文献
252.
Wolff NJ Darlington AS Hunfeld JA Tharner A Van Ijzendoorn MH Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ Moll HA Jaddoe VW Hofman A Verhulst FC Passchier J Tiemeier H 《Infant behavior & development》2011,34(2):293-302
Objective
This study examined the effects of attachment and temperament on infant distress during venipuncture.Method
The study was embedded in the Generation R Study, a prospective population-based study. Two different research procedures (i.e., blood sampling and the Ainsworth Strange Situation Procedure) yielded measures of venipuncture distress and attachment security and disorganization in 246 infants aged 14 months. Four temperament traits (distress to limitations, fear, recovery from distress, and sadness) were assessed using the maternally reported Infant Behavior Questionnaire - Revised, at the age of 6 months.Results
There were no differences between mean levels of distress during venipuncture in infants classified as having insecure attachment, but there was a trend for disorganized attachment. The temperament traits were not related to distress. However, children with a disorganized attachment and higher temperamental fear had more venipuncture distress.Conclusion
When different risk factors are present simultaneously, infant distress is heightened. 相似文献253.
This article reports the results of a survey of Canadian Chinese pastors (N 20) regarding their experiences and attitudes related to pastoral counseling. It identifies common concerns encountered during pastoral counseling, as well as key areas of church concerns. Results of the survey indicate that pastoral counseling may be more important than is currently recognized by Chinese pastors, particularly in key areas of marriage, family life, and general relationship issues. The authors discuss possible implications of the survey relative to training and equipping Chinese pastors to engage more in pastoral counseling as an integral part of their parish ministry. 相似文献
254.
255.
This review evaluates the effectiveness of peer mediation in increasing the social behaviors of isolated children in school settings. Studies indicate that peer assisted interventions can produce increases in social behavior and the generalization of treatment effects across a variety of situations. Discussion also focuses on ethical considerations when peers serve as behavior change agents, maximizing generalization effects, and suggestions for future research. 相似文献
256.
257.
Ris L Capron B Sclavons C Liégeois JF Seutin V Godaux E 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2007,14(6):390-399
In area CA1 of hippocampal slices, a single 1-sec train of 100-Hz stimulation generally triggers a short-lasting long-term potentiation (S-LTP) of 1–2 h. Here, we found that when such a train was applied 45 min after application of the small conductance Ca2+-activated K+ (SK) channel blocker apamin, it induced a long-lasting LTP (L-LTP) of several hours, instead of an S-LTP. Apamin-induced SK channel blockage is known to resist washing. Nevertheless, the aforementioned effect is not a mere delayed effect; it is metaplastic. Indeed, when a single train was delivered to the Schaffer’s collaterals during apamin application, it induced an S-LTP, like in the control situation. At the moment of this LTP induction (15th min of apamin application), the SK channel blockage was nevertheless complete. Indeed, at that time, under the influence of apamin, the amplitude of the series of field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) triggered by a stimulation train was increased. We found that the metaplastic effect of apamin on LTP was crucially dependent on the NO-synthase pathway, whereas the efficacy of the NMDA receptors was not modified at the time of its occurrence. We also found that apamin produced an increase in paired-pulse facilitation not during, but after, the application of the drug. Finally, we found that the induction of each of these two metaplastic phenomena was mediated by NMDA receptors. A speculative unitary hypothesis to explain these phenomena is proposed. 相似文献
258.
Contribution of somesthetic information to the perception of body orientation in the pitch dimension
Lionel Bringoux Vincent Nougier Ludovic Marin Pierre-Alain Barraud Christian Raphel 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2003,56(5):909-923
This study investigated the contribution of otolithic and somesthetic inputs in the perception of body orientation when pitching at very slow velocities. In Experiment 1, the subjects' task was to indicate their subjective postural vertical, in two different conditions of body restriction, starting from different angles of body tilt. In the strapped condition, subjects were attached onto a platform by means of large straps. In the body cast condition, subjects were completely immobilized in a depressurized system, which attenuates gravity-based somesthetic cues. Results showed that the condition of body restriction and the initial tilt largely influenced the subjective postural vertical. In Experiment 2, subjects were displaced from a vertical position and had to detect the direction of body tilts. Results showed that the threshold for the perception of body tilt was higher when subjects were immobilized in the body cast and when they were tilted backward. Experiment 3 replicated the same protocol from a supine starting position. Compared to results of Experiment 2, the threshold for the perception of body tilt decreased significantly. Overall, these data suggested that gravity-based somesthetic cues are more informative than otolithic cues for the perception of a quasi-static body orientation. 相似文献
259.
260.
W. Todd Maddox Brian D. Glass Jeffrey B. O’Brien J. Vincent Filoteo F. Gregory Ashby 《Psychological research》2010,74(2):219-236
The category shift literature suggests that rule-based classification, an important form of explicit learning, is mediated
by two separate learned associations: a stimulus-to-label association that associates stimuli and category labels, and a label-to-response
association that associates category labels and responses. Three experiments investigate whether information–integration classification,
an important form of implicit learning, is also mediated by two separate learned associations. Participants were trained on
a rule-based or an information–integration categorization task and then the association between stimulus and category label,
or between category label and response location was altered. For rule-based categories, and in line with previous research,
breaking the association between stimulus and category label caused more interference than breaking the association between
category label and response location. However, no differences in recovery rate emerged. For information–integration categories,
breaking the association between stimulus and category label caused more interference and led to greater recovery than breaking
the association between category label and response location. These results provide evidence that information–integration
category learning is mediated by separate stimulus-to-label and label-to-response associations. Implications for the neurobiological
basis of these two learned associations are discussed. 相似文献