全文获取类型
收费全文 | 515篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
540篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 22篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 81篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 24篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 35篇 |
2007年 | 26篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 2篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
1950年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有540条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Esther Menaker Ph.D. 《Journal of religion and health》1994,33(1):81-88
This paper begins by defining the selfobject and elucidating its role in the structuring of the self for each unique individual.
The self becomes a repository of the memory images of emotional experiences with significant others. This process of internalization
through which the self is continually nourished, and which enables it to grow, goes on throughout life. However, as a case
example illustrates, the ability to internalize a selfobject depends on the ability to receive and to give love. Once the
selfobject is incorporated into the self of “an other” it is perpetuated in another, is perpetuated, analogous to the transmission
of genetic material. Each individual carries others within himself or herself and has the potentiality for continuing to live
within the self of another. This is a kind of immortality.
She is the author of many articles and books, most recentlyAppointment in Vienna (St. Martin's Press). 相似文献
92.
93.
94.
Corianna E. Sichel Esther Burson Shabnam Javdani Erin B. Godfrey 《American journal of community psychology》2019,63(3-4):405-417
Each year approximately 48,000 youth are incarcerated in residential placement facilities (YRFs) in the United States. The limited existing literature addressing the workforce in these settings paints a complicated picture. The YRF workforce is highly motivated to work with legal system involved youth. However, YRF staff report high rates of burnout, job fatigue, and work‐related stress. The current paper proposes solutions to persistent problems faced by staff in these settings by integrating literature from criminology, organizational psychology, trauma‐informed care, and community psychology. In doing so, we highlight previously overlooked aspects of intervention for trauma‐organized settings and respond to recent calls for community psychologists to take a more active role in the adaptation of trauma‐informed care in community settings. We conclude by advancing three recommendations, drawn from setting‐level theory and inspired by the principles of trauma‐informed care, to transform YRFs. 相似文献
95.
Building on social exchange theory, this study examines the relationship between perceptions of organizational politics and employee performance and interpersonal skills. We hypothesized that perceptions of organizational politics create an imbalance in the exchange relationship between employee and organization, which leads to organizational deviance. We also hypothesized that supervisors may attempt to rebalance the exchange relationship by providing lower performance and interpersonal skills ratings. Finally, we suggest that politically skilled employees avoid an increase in negative ratings. The present study demonstrates that perceptions of organizational politics may lead to negative employee behaviors and reduced supervisors’ ratings in an effort to rebalance the exchange relationship. In addition, politically skilled employees may avoid increased negative ratings when politics perceptions are high. 相似文献
96.
97.
Situating interventions to bridge the intention–behaviour gap: A framework for recruiting nonconscious processes for behaviour change 下载免费PDF全文
This paper presents a situated cognition framework for creating social psychological interventions to bridge the intention–behaviour gap and illustrates this framework by reviewing examples from the domains of health behaviour, environmental behaviour, stereotyping, and aggression. A recurrent problem in behaviour change is the fact that often, intentions are not translated into behaviour, causing the so‐called intention–behaviour gap. Here, it is argued that this happens when situational cues trigger situated conceptualizations, such as habits, impulses, hedonic goals, or stereotypical associations, which can then guide behaviour automatically. To be effective in changing such automatic effects, behaviour change interventions can attempt to change situational cues through cueing interventions such as priming, nudging, upstream policy interventions, or reminders of social norms. Alternatively, behaviour change interventions can attempt to change the underlying situated conceptualizations through training interventions, such as behavioural inhibition training, mindfulness training, or implementation intentions. Examples of situated behaviour change interventions of both types will be discussed across domains, along with recommendations to situate interventions more strongly and thus enhance their effectiveness to change automatic behaviour. Finally, the discussion addresses the difference between tailoring and situating interventions, issues of generalization and long‐term effectiveness, and avenues for further research. 相似文献
98.
Sánchez-Carracedo D Barrada JR López-Guimerà G Fauquet J Almenara CA Trepat E 《Body image》2012,9(1):163-171
The aims of the present study were: (1) to assess the factor structure of the SATAQ-3 in Spanish secondary-school students by means of exploratory factor analysis (EFA), confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) models; and (2) to study its invariance by sex and school grade. ESEM is a technique that has been proposed for the analysis of internal structure that overcomes some of the limitations of EFA and CFA. Participants were 1559 boys and girls in grades seventh to tenth. The results support the four-factor solution of the original version, and reveal that the best fit was obtained with ESEM, excluding Item 20 and with correlated uniqueness between reverse-keyed items. Our version shows invariance by sex and grade. The differences between scores of different groups are in the expected direction, and support the validity of the questionnaire. We recommend a version excluding Item 20 and without reverse-keyed items. 相似文献
99.
Esther Brendel Patricia R. DeLucia Heiko Hecht Ryan L. Stacy Jeff T. Larsen 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2012,74(5):979-987
The ability to estimate the time remaining until collision occurs with an approaching object (time-to-collision, TTC) is crucial for any mobile animal. In the present study, we report three experiments examining whether higher level cognitive factors, represented by affective value of approaching objects, could affect judgments of TTC. A theory of TTC estimates based purely on the optical variable tau does not predict an influence of the affective value of an approaching object. In Experiments 1 and 2, we compared TTC estimates of threatening and neutral pictures that approached our participants on a screen and disappeared from view before a collision would have occurred. Images were taken from the International Affective Picture System. Threatening pictures??in particular, the picture of a frontal attack??were judged to collide earlier than neutral pictures. In Experiment 3, the approaching stimuli were faces with different emotional expressions. TTC tended to be underestimated for angry faces. We discuss these results, considering the roles of affective and cognitive mechanisms modulating TTC estimation and general time perception. 相似文献
100.
A striking and characteristic feature of the restricting subtype of anorexia nervosa (AN) is that they are extremely successful in regulating their food intake in a destructive manner. A possible explanation for the persistent character of their restricted food intake could be a loss of the motivational saliency of food in restricting AN patients. The present study aimed to investigate motivational orientation for food in the restricting subtype of anorexia nervosa with an indirect performance-based measure and a self-report measure of craving. An indirect approach avoidance task was administered to measure automatic approach tendencies for high-fat and low-fat food in restricting adolescent AN-like patients (n=89) and a group of healthy adolescents (n=76). As predicted, restricting AN-like patients showed less automatic motivational orientation toward food than healthy controls. In a similar vein, AN-like patients reported less craving for food than the group of healthy controls. The loss of an automatic motivational orientation toward food together with the deliberate strategy to avoid food might help explain the ability of restricting AN-like patients to regulate their food intake. 相似文献