全文获取类型
收费全文 | 773篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
784篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 20篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 27篇 |
2019年 | 33篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 40篇 |
2016年 | 42篇 |
2015年 | 35篇 |
2014年 | 38篇 |
2013年 | 91篇 |
2012年 | 39篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 22篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 33篇 |
2007年 | 30篇 |
2006年 | 27篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 20篇 |
2003年 | 18篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1951年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有784条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
541.
Amanda L. Farrell Robert D. Keppel Victoria B. Titterington 《Journal of Investigative Psychology & Offender Profiling》2013,10(3):268-288
Serial murder is a genre of crime that has received a great deal of media and academic attention, yet, serial murder committed by women has only begun to garner a portion of this attention within the last 15 years. This study examines the reliability of Kelleher and Kelleher's classification rubric, the only classification system developed for female serial murderers, as well as considering Hickey's classification of serial offenders by location. Other variables associated with homicide research have also been examined to determine their roles in both these crimes and classifying offenders. The current research will demonstrate that offender motivation is not an ideal basis for the classification of female serial murderers, who tend to defy simple or singular classification within existing typologies. The other variables analysed indicate that victim–offender relationship and victim approach are important to understanding these female offenders and their crimes, as well as the possible development of more accurate classification systems. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
542.
Professor Graham F. Wagstaff Jacqueline Wheatcroft Jon C. Cole Joana Brunas-Wagstaff Victoria Blackmore Andrew Pilkington 《Journal of Cognitive Psychology》2013,25(4):828-846
Tasks that, in the working memory and neuropsychological literature, have been related to executive and frontal processing, and nonexecutive and more posterior brain processing, bear distinct similarities with the kinds of tasks that, in the social psychological literature, have been associated with social inhibition and facilitation, respectively. Accordingly, a cognitive-neuropsychological model of social inhibition and facilitation is proposed whereby the presence of others engages the executive and frontal systems, and facilitates the function of systems active in more automatic, nonexecutive processing. In two experiments, the tasks most associated with executive and frontal processing (phonemic switches and answers to complex questions) showed evidence of social inhibition, whereas those more associated with nonexecutive and more posterior temporal processing (phonemic clusters and confidence–accuracy correlations), showed some evidence of social facilitation. Implications are discussed. 相似文献
543.
Domicele Jonauskaite;Déborah Epicoco;Abdulrahman S. Al-rasheed;John Jamir Benzon R. Aruta;Victoria Bogushevskaya;Sanne G. Brederoo;Violeta Corona;Sergejs Fomins;Alena Gizdic;Yulia A. Griber;Jelena Havelka;Marco Hirnstein;George John;Daniela S. Jopp;Bodil Karlsson;Nikos Konstantinou;Éric Laurent;Lynn Marquardt;Philip C. Mefoh;Daniel Oberfeld;Marietta Papadatou-Pastou;Corinna M. Perchtold-Stefan;Giulia F. M. Spagnulo;Aygun Sultanova;Takumi Tanaka;Ma. Criselda Tengco-Pacquing;Mari Uusküla;Grażyna Wąsowicz;Christine Mohr; 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2024,115(2):275-305
As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour–emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour–emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour–emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour. 相似文献
544.
In a follow-up to a previously published study (Epstein, Schmidt, & Warfel, 2008), an ethnically-diverse sample of 13,578 people in 47 countries (mainly the United States and Canada) took an online test that measures 4 trainable competencies that have been shown to enhance creative expression in individuals. The new study confirmed that the test has high internal-consistency reliability and reasonably strong predictive validity. Competency scores were good predictors of self-reported frequency of creative expression and moderately good predictors of life satisfaction and professional success. Scores were also substantially higher for people who had had creativity training, and more training was associated with higher scores. Small but statistically significant differences in scores were found for gender (women outscoring men) and race/ethnicity. Of the 4 competencies examined, capturing (preserves new ideas as they occur) proved to be the best predictor of self-reported creative expression, followed by challenging (takes on difficult tasks), broadening (expands one's skills and knowledge), and surrounding (arranges stimulating physical and social environments). 相似文献
545.
Abraham Wandersman Victoria H. Chien Jason Katz 《American journal of community psychology》2012,50(3-4):460-461
Special Issue: Advances in Bridging Research and Practice Using the Interactive System Framework for Dissemination and Implementation; Guest Editors: Abraham Wandersman, Paul Flaspohler, Catherine A. Lesesne, Richard Puddy; Action Editor: Emilie Phillips Smith 相似文献
546.
Abraham Wandersman Victoria H. Chien Jason Katz 《American journal of community psychology》2012,50(3-4):445-459
An individual or organization that sets out to implement an innovation (e.g., a new technology, program, or policy) generally requires support. In the Interactive Systems Framework for Dissemination and Implementation, a Support System should work with Delivery Systems (national, state and/or local entities such as health and human service organizations, community-based organizations, schools) to enhance their capacity for quality implementation of innovations. The literature on the Support ystem has been under-researched and under-developed. This article begins to conceptualize theory, research, and action for an evidence-based system for innovation support (EBSIS). EBSIS describes key priorities for strengthening the science and practice of support. The major goal of EBSIS is to enhance the research and practice of support in order to build capacity in the Delivery System for implementing innovations with quality, and thereby, help the Delivery System achieve outcomes. EBSIS is guided by a logic model that includes four key support components: tools, training, technical assistance, and quality assurance/quality improvement. EBSIS uses the Getting To Outcomes approach to accountability to aid the identification and synthesis of concepts, tools, and evidence for support. We conclude with some discussion of the current status of EBSIS and possible next steps, including the development of collaborative researcher-practitioner-funder-consumer partnerships to accelerate accumulation of knowledge on the Support System. 相似文献
547.
Early findings from Broca and Wernicke led to the classical view of hemispheric specialization, where the main idea relates to left-hemisphere language capabilities compared to right-hemisphere visual capabilities. Federmeier and Benjamin (2005) have suggested that semantic encoding for verbal information in the right hemisphere can be more effective when memory demands are higher. In light of this, our main goal was to study the effect of retention level of verbal information on hemispheric processes. However, regarding the cross-linguistic differences in orthography and their subsequent effects on word recognition (Frost, Katz, & Bentin, 1987), our intent was also to test prior predictions of Federmeier and Benjamin (2005) for a \"shallow\" orthography language, where words have a clear correspondence between graphemes and phonemes, as opposed to English, which is a \"deep\" orthography language. Portuguese concrete nouns were selected. The participants were submitted to a visual half-field word presentation using a continuous recognition memory paradigm. The retention level included 1, 2, 4, 8, 20 or 40 words. Results showed that recognition accuracy was higher for words studied in the right visual field, compared to those studied in the left visual field, when the retention interval included 2, 4, or 20 words. No significant differences were found for the remaining intervals. Further analysis on accuracy data for intermediate retention levels showed that recognition accuracy was higher for the 2 words retention level than for the levels including 4, 8, or 20 words; it was higher for left-hemisphere encoding as well. Our results also indicated that reaction times were slower for left-hemisphere encoding and for the 40 words retention level when compared to that of 20 words. In summary, the current results are in partial agreement with those of Federmeier and Benjamin (2005) and suggest different hemispheric memory strategies for the semantic encoding of verbal information. 相似文献
548.
Eric L. Kohatsu Rodolfo Victoria Andrew Lau Michelle Flores Andrea Salazar 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2011,89(1):63-72
The purpose of this study was to examine to what extent both racial identity and color‐blind racial attitudes help explain anti‐Asian prejudice across different socioracial groups. Participants of color from a culturally diverse West Coast university were surveyed (N = 260). Hierarchical regression analyses showed that resistance racial identity and color‐blind attitudes predicted anti‐Asian prejudice. Results further validated racial identity theory as a viable tool for understanding interracial relations among Asians and other socioracial minority groups. 相似文献
549.
550.
The identification of reliable and valid means for evaluating the effectiveness of school-based treatments and completing
diagnostic evaluations of middle school aged students are needed. The present study examined the inter-rater agreement of
teacher ratings and the relationship between ratings and observational data in a middle school setting. The data are interpreted
in the context of differences between a secondary and elementary school setting. Teacher ratings and observational data were
collected regularly over the course of two academic years for middle school students diagnosed with ADHD. The results indicate
low rates of inter-rater agreement as well as low rates of agreement between teachers and observational data, and between
observational data collected in different classrooms. Inter-rater agreement was lowest in late fall and gradually increased
over the second half of the year. Implications for conducting treatment outcome evaluations of school-based treatment programs
and diagnostic evaluations are discussed. 相似文献