全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40004篇 |
免费 | 638篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
40644篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 481篇 |
2019年 | 617篇 |
2018年 | 799篇 |
2017年 | 856篇 |
2016年 | 882篇 |
2015年 | 609篇 |
2014年 | 745篇 |
2013年 | 3102篇 |
2012年 | 1331篇 |
2011年 | 1332篇 |
2010年 | 873篇 |
2009年 | 807篇 |
2008年 | 1168篇 |
2007年 | 1196篇 |
2006年 | 1061篇 |
2005年 | 926篇 |
2004年 | 872篇 |
2003年 | 828篇 |
2002年 | 845篇 |
2001年 | 1178篇 |
2000年 | 1135篇 |
1999年 | 887篇 |
1998年 | 390篇 |
1997年 | 393篇 |
1996年 | 357篇 |
1995年 | 361篇 |
1993年 | 355篇 |
1992年 | 741篇 |
1991年 | 657篇 |
1990年 | 709篇 |
1989年 | 606篇 |
1988年 | 642篇 |
1987年 | 607篇 |
1986年 | 612篇 |
1985年 | 556篇 |
1984年 | 511篇 |
1983年 | 490篇 |
1982年 | 357篇 |
1979年 | 575篇 |
1978年 | 400篇 |
1975年 | 462篇 |
1974年 | 500篇 |
1973年 | 533篇 |
1972年 | 402篇 |
1971年 | 395篇 |
1970年 | 354篇 |
1969年 | 407篇 |
1968年 | 474篇 |
1967年 | 431篇 |
1966年 | 357篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
281.
Patrick C. L. Heaven 《Personality and individual differences》1984,5(6):741-742
Previous research among university students has identified four classes of assertive behavior, namely, Directiveness, Social Assertiveness, Defense of One's Rights and Interests and Independence. Scores on the Lorr and More (1980) Assertiveness Inventory, obtained from a general population sample, were subjected to Varimax rotatio. Results revealed only three classes of assertiveness. It is argued that Directiveness may be inferred from various forms of assertiveness, rather than being a separate and distinct class of assertive behavior. 相似文献
282.
This experiment examined whether structural considerations, that is the functional closeness of limb control centres (Kinsbourne and Hicks 1978), have a role to play in common processing models of psychological refractoriness. A typical double stimulation paradigm was used (Kantowitz 1974). On experimental trials, subjects responded with the right hand (RT1) to an auditory signal (0.80 probability). After a varied interstimulus interval (ISI) of 100, 200, 300 or 700 msec, one of three lights corresponding to one of the other limbs provided the signal for the second response (RT2). Control trials for RT1 and RT2 involved both stimuli, but subjects were required to make only one response. Results indicated that experimental RT1 was lengthened considerably over control values. While typical double stimulation effects were found at short ISIs, that is RT2 was lengthened, RT2 at long ISIs was actually faster than double stimulation control values. Although the RT results were not in a pattern that would be predicted by functional distance (Kinsbourne and Hicks 1978), within subject correlations of RT1 and RT2 by limb condition suggest that structural considerations may have a role to play. While within subject correlations were positive at all levels of ISI, they decreased as the interval between the two signals increased. The implications of these results for common processing models are discussed. 相似文献
283.
John B Pryor Teri L Kott Gregory R Bovee 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1984,20(3):246-262
The following question is addressed: How do multiple, simultaneously present cognitive structures influence the representation and recall of social information? In an empirical study examining both free and cued recall, we found the variable information redundancy to influence both the organization and accuracy of subjects' recollections of trait-related behaviors. Redundancy was defined in terms of the degree of person/trait overlap in a social information ensemble. Some evidence indicated that this effect is attributable to an increase in the discriminability of the organizational structures during encoding. The implications of these findings for understanding the formation of integrated cognitive representations of individuals and the spontaneous use of traits in social information processing are discussed. 相似文献
284.
285.
Semantic and visual memory codes in learning disabled readers 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
H L Swanson 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1984,37(1):124-140
Two experiments investigated whether learning disabled readers' impaired recall is due to multiple coding deficiencies. In Experiment 1, learning disabled and skilled readers viewed nonsense pictures without names or with either relevant or irrelevant names with respect to the distinctive characteristics of the picture. Both types of names improved recall of nondisabled readers, while learning disabled readers exhibited better recall for unnamed pictures. No significant difference in recall was found between name training (relevant, irrelevant) conditions within reading groups. In Experiment 2, both reading groups participated in recall training for complex visual forms labeled with unrelated words, hierarchically related words, or without labels. A subsequent reproduction transfer task showed a facilitation in performance in skilled readers due to labeling, with learning disabled readers exhibiting better reproduction for unnamed pictures. Measures of output organization (clustering) indicated that recall is related to the development of superordinate categories. The results suggest that learning disabled children's reading difficulties are due to an inability to activate a semantic representation that interconnects visual and verbal codes. 相似文献
286.
Ralph L. Elkins 《Behaviour research and therapy》1984,22(2):179-186
Inquiries into the longevity of illness-induced aversions (TAs) in animals are relevant to consummatory-aversion (CA) treatments of human alcoholism. The range of nausea reactions that accompanied the relapses of some alcoholics who had acquired alcohol aversions during covertsensitization (verbal aversion) alcoholism treatment has implicated CA forgetting as one probable contributor to recidivism. CA forgetting is operationalized as aversion diminution during postconditioning periods in which Ss abstain from contact with the target substance. TAs of varying strengths were induced in groups of Sprague-Dawley rats that received low, medium or high doses of the illness-inducing drug cyclophosphamide following saccharin-solution ingestion. TA retention was assessed following saccharin-free intervals of 2–40 days. Each Ss' retention interval was followed by 30 days of two-bottle preference testing, thereby additionally permitting an assessment of TA extinction following differing degrees of TA forgetting. Low-dose Ss displayed moderate strength TAs that were forgotten within 20 days and that had little resistance to extinction when testing began shortly after conditioning. Medium-dose Ss displayed stronger TAs having greater resistance to both forgetting and extinction. Unlike these low- and medium-dose TAs, high-dose TAs were impervious to aversion degradation as a result of forgetting. This finding is interpreted as supporting the attempted induction of intense nausea during covert-sensitization and chemical aversion (emetic therapy) alcoholism treatment. Other related conditioning procedures that may contribute to effective treatment are also discussed. 相似文献
287.
Michael L. Feldstein Henry T. Davis 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》1984,37(1):49-61
In this paper we discuss a method for assessing agreement among raters who are scoring the number of times, a specified event occurs. In such cases, it seems reasonable to define agreement in terms of raters' behaviours in correctly identifying responses which have in fact occurred, and in their falsely counting responses which have not. We exploit the discrete nature of the response variable, and examine a class of models for mean response assuming an underlying Poisson distribution. Test statistics are given for deciding on the applicability of the models, and whether there is agreement with respect to correctly detecting responses, as well as with falsely scoring responses. 相似文献
288.
289.
Joanne Benjamin-Bauman Maxin L. Reiss Jon S. Bailey 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1984,17(3):295-301
We examined the effect of reducing the interval between a patient's call for an appointment and the appointment itself. In Experiment 1, patients calling a family planning unit of a public health department were assigned appointments within either 1 or 3 weeks of their call. Data on patient “shows” and “no-shows” were recorded weekly for 6 weeks. Show rates for those in the 1-week appointment group were significantly higher than those in the 3-week group. In Experiment 2, patients were assigned to appointment dates either the next operating clinic day (next-day group) or 2 weeks from the call date (2-week group). Show rates for those in the next-day group were significantly better than show rates for patients in the 2-week group. Clinic productivity, time spent with patients, and consumer satisfaction were also assessed. Implications for appointment scheduling are discussed. 相似文献
290.
RILEY HARVILL EDWARD E. JACOBS ROBERT L. MASSON 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》1984,62(5):273-275
The use of props as counseling aids can add a new dimension to any counselor's current repertoire of skills. 相似文献