全文获取类型
收费全文 | 185篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
189篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 15篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 18篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2009年 | 4篇 |
2008年 | 6篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 3篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有189条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Animal Cognition - The commentary by Gallup and Anderson (Anim Cogn https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-021-01538-9 , 2021) on the original article by Baragli, Scopa, Maglieri, and Palagi (Anim Cogn... 相似文献
73.
The relationship between type of stimulus (numeric and verbal) and type of precocity (mathematical and verbal) was examined in tasks designed to tap three aspects of working memory: encoding, capacity, and manipulation of information. The tasks included semantic categorization, odd-even categorization, recall of five-item lists after semantic categorization, and recall of items in a continuous paired-associates task. Correlations between task performance and the mathematical and verbal portions of the Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT-M and SAT-V) were computed for gifted youth. There were no sex differences in the performance measures or in the pattern of correlations between performance and SAT scores. The analysis revealed positive relationships between SAT-M scores and numeric categorization latency, recall in the continuous paired-associate task with words and digits, and recall of digit lists. SAT-V scores were related only to word recall in the continuous paired-associate task and recall of word lists. In the working-memory tasks used, mathematical precocity is more strongly related to performance than is verbal precocity. The relationship is especially strong with numeric stimuli, even when the numeric stimuli are simply items to be remembered. The relationship between type of stimulus and type of precocity suggests underlying differences between verbally and mathematically precocious youth in how different types of stimuli are represented in memory. 相似文献
74.
Veronica J. Dark 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(4):299-308
In two experiments, semantic analysis of prime words was measured in terms of facilitation in naming a semantically related target word. Targets were degraded but gradually clarified until the subject named them. Subjects reported the prime after naming the target. Experiment 1 used semantic associates as primes at a 50-msec prime-target stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). Experiment 2 used both semantic-associate and identity primes at a 1,000-msec prime-target SOA. Reported primes showed facilitation in both experiments, whereas unreported primes did not. It appears that primes that undergo enough analysis to facilitate target processing are also available for conscious report. However, retroactive priming in both experiments showed that target processing also had an impact on prime reportability. The interdependence of priming and prime reportability disallows a straightforward interpretation of the origin of the facilitation. 相似文献
75.
Results from research with the flanker task have been used to argue both that flankers are identified without attention and that flanker identificatin requires attention. In three experiments, we addressed this issue by examining flanker recall. In Experiment 1, we manipulated flanker redundancy, a variable that could influence attention to the flankers, in order to determine whether it affected the magnitude of the flanker effect, the magnitude of flanker recall, or both. Redundancy did not influence the flanker effect, and recall was high in both conditions, suggesting that the flankers were attended. The recall results contradicted those of Miller (1987), the only other study that we could find in which flanker recall was used. In Experiments 2 and 3, we examined differences between Miller’s procedure and ours. Although flanker recall was lower when open-ended rather than forced recall was used and when the flanker task was made more complicated, flanker recall remained well above chance in all conditions. The data strongly suggest that in the typical correlated flanker task there is a failure of selective attention at some point during processing such that flankers receive attentional processing. 相似文献
76.
Negative priming (NP) occurs when responses are slower because the targets were distractors on the preceding trial. Word-naming
NP occurs only with words that have been presented repeatedly as targets; novel words do not show NP. The activation-inhibition
explanation is that representations of repeated-word distractors are activated already and must be inhibited; the inhibition
carries over to the next trial. If this explanation is correct, novel-word NP should occur if the word is semantically primed
(thus activating its representation) before it occurs as a distractor. In two experiments, there was NP for words from a repeated
set, and the magnitude of NP increased when the same word could occur as a target on consecutive trials. There was positive,
rather than negative, priming for novel-word targets that had been semantically primed as distractors. Either the activation
from semantic priming was not sufficiently strong to require inhibition, or the activation-inhibition hypothesis does not
refer to activation of conceptual representations. 相似文献
77.
Social Psychology of Education - The present study builds on and adds to a growing body of literature suggesting that teachers’ mindsets predict students’ classroom experiences. We... 相似文献
78.
79.
The brief aggression questionnaire: psychometric and behavioral evidence for an efficient measure of trait aggression 下载免费PDF全文
Gregory D. Webster C. Nathan DeWall Richard S. Pond Jr. Timothy Deckman Peter K. Jonason Bonnie M. Le Austin Lee Nichols Tatiana Orozco Schember Laura C. Crysel Benjamin S. Crosier C. Veronica Smith E. Layne Paddock John B. Nezlek Lee A. Kirkpatrick Angela D. Bryan Renée J. Bator 《Aggressive behavior》2014,40(2):120-139
80.
Samuel C. Andrew Clint J. Perry Andrew B. Barron Katherine Berthon Veronica Peralta Ken Cheng 《Animal cognition》2014,17(5):1177-1186
If animals are trained with two similar stimuli such that one is rewarding (S+) and one punishing (S?), then following training animals show a greatest preference not for the S+, but for a novel stimulus that is slightly more different from the S? than the S+ is. This peak shift phenomenon has been widely reported for vertebrates and has recently been demonstrated for bumblebees and honey bees. To explore the nature of peak shift in invertebrates further, here we examined the properties of peak shift in honey bees trained in a free-flight olfactory learning assay. Hexanal and heptanol were mixed in different ratios to create a continuum of odour stimuli. Bees were trained to artificial flowers such that one odour mixture was rewarded with 2 molar sucrose (S+), and one punished with distasteful quinine (S?). After training, bees were given a non-rewarded preference test with five different mixtures of hexanal and heptanol. Following training bees’ maximal preference was for an odour mixture slightly more distinct from the S? than the trained S+. This effect was not seen if bees were initially trained with two distinct odours, replicating the classic features of peak shift reported for vertebrates. We propose a conceptual model of how peak shift might occur in honey bees. We argue that peak shift does not require any higher level of processing than the known olfactory learning circuitry of the bee brain and suggest that peak shift is a very general feature of discrimination learning. 相似文献