全文获取类型
收费全文 | 296篇 |
免费 | 8篇 |
专业分类
304篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 11篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 4篇 |
2013年 | 37篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 10篇 |
2010年 | 3篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 12篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有304条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
The challenges of specifying a complex and individualized treatment model and measuring fidelity thereto are described, using multisystemic therapy (MST) as an example. Relations between therapist adherence to MST principles and instrumental and ultimate outcome variables are examined, as are relations between clinical supervision and therapist adherence. The findings provide modest support for the associations between MST adherence measures and instrumental and ultimate outcomes. Results also show that adherence can be altered when clinical supervision and adherence monitoring procedures are fortified. The modest associations between adherence measures and youth outcomes argue for further refinement and validation of the MST adherence measure, especially in light of the well-established effectiveness of MST with challenging clinical populations and the increasing dissemination of MST programs. 相似文献
102.
According to Crain and Nakayama (1987) , when forming complex yes/no questions, children do not make errors such as Is the boy who smoking is crazy? because they have innate knowledge of structure dependence and so will not move the auxiliary from the relative clause. However, simple recurrent networks are also able to avoid such errors, on the basis of surface distributional properties of the input ( Lewis & Elman, 2001 ; Reali & Christiansen, 2005 ). Two new elicited production studies revealed that (a) children occasionally produce structure‐dependence errors and (b) the pattern of children's auxiliary‐doubling errors (Is the boy who is smoking is crazy?) suggests a sensitivity to surface co‐occurrence patterns in the input. This article concludes that current data do not provide any support for the claim that structure dependence is an innate constraint, and that it is possible that children form a structure‐dependent grammar on the basis of exposure to input that exhibits this property. 相似文献
103.
Stephen G. Weinrach Albert Ellis Catharine MacLaren Raymond Di Giuseppe Ann Vernon Janet Wolfe Ruth Malkinson Wouter Backx 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2001,79(3):259-268
Eight experts in Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) provided personal examples of their own successes and failures in applying REBT to themselves. The experts actively talked to themselves both rationally and irrationally. Understandably, there were far more shoulds, oughts, musts, and have to's in the narratives in which the experts described when they failed to use REBT than when they succeeded in using REBT. Rational self‐talk was more prevalent in the examples of how REBT was successfully used by the experts. 相似文献
104.
105.
106.
Starting from the assumption that one can literally perceive someone's anger in their face, I argue that this would not be possible if what is perceived is a static facial signature of their anger. There is a product–process distinction in talk of facial expression, and I argue that one can see anger in someone's facial expression only if this is understood to be a process rather than a product. 相似文献
107.
The present study investigated how children learn that some verbs may appear in the figure-locative but not the ground-locative construction (e.g., Lisa poured water into the cup; *Lisa poured the cup with water), with some showing the opposite pattern (e.g., *Bart filled water into the cup; Bart filled the cup with water), and others appearing in both (Lisa sprayed water onto the flowers; Lisa sprayed the flowers with water). Grammatical acceptability judgments were obtained for the use of each of 142 locative verbs (60 for children) in each sentence type. Overall, and for each age group individually, the judgment data were best explained by a model that included ratings of the extent to which each verb exhibits both the broad- and narrow-range semantic properties of the figure- and ground-locative constructions (relating mainly to manner and end-state respectively; Pinker, 1989) and the statistical-learning measure of overall verb frequency (entrenchment; Braine & Brooks, 1995). A second statistical-learning measure, frequency in each of the two locative constructions (pre-emption; Goldberg, 1995), was found to have no additional dissociable effect. We conclude by drawing together various theoretical proposals to arrive at a possible account of how semantics and statistics interact in the retreat from overgeneralization. 相似文献
108.
110.
Vernon Carter 《International Journal of Philosophical Studies》2013,21(5):687-690