首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   19924篇
  免费   2130篇
  国内免费   15篇
  2021年   203篇
  2020年   305篇
  2019年   605篇
  2018年   605篇
  2017年   717篇
  2016年   713篇
  2015年   597篇
  2014年   586篇
  2013年   1790篇
  2012年   816篇
  2011年   826篇
  2010年   599篇
  2009年   553篇
  2008年   649篇
  2007年   555篇
  2006年   492篇
  2005年   442篇
  2004年   415篇
  2003年   412篇
  2002年   418篇
  2001年   776篇
  2000年   763篇
  1999年   497篇
  1998年   187篇
  1997年   179篇
  1996年   141篇
  1995年   137篇
  1993年   139篇
  1992年   364篇
  1991年   327篇
  1990年   347篇
  1989年   338篇
  1988年   319篇
  1987年   291篇
  1986年   313篇
  1985年   298篇
  1984年   236篇
  1983年   200篇
  1979年   190篇
  1978年   161篇
  1975年   187篇
  1974年   241篇
  1973年   197篇
  1972年   203篇
  1971年   167篇
  1970年   149篇
  1969年   198篇
  1968年   202篇
  1967年   161篇
  1966年   182篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Whether people compete or cooperate with each other has consequences for their own performance and that of organizations. To explain why people compete or cooperate, previous research has focused on two main factors: situational outcome structures and personality types. Here, we propose that—above and beyond these two factors—situational cues, such as the format in which people receive feedback, strongly affect whether they act competitively, cooperatively, or individualistically. Results of a laboratory experiment support our theorizing: After receiving ranking feedback, both students and experienced managers treated group situations with cooperative outcome structures as competitive and were in consequence willing to forgo guaranteed financial gains to pursue a—financially irrelevant—better rank. Conversely, in dilemma situations, feedback based on the joint group outcome led to more cooperation than ranking feedback. Our study contributes to research on competition, cooperation, interdependence theory, forced ranking, and the design of information environments.  相似文献   
32.
Recent studies demonstrating that multiple meanings of an ambiguous word are initially accessed even when only one reading is syntactically appropriate with the preceding context can be criticized on at least two grounds. First, many of the syntactic contexts used were not truly restrictive, and, secondly, subjects may not have had time to integrate the context before processing the ambiguous word. In the present study, subjects listened to a sentence ending in an ambiguous word and then made a lexical decision to a target related to either the appropriate or inappropriate reading. Contexts were completely restrictive, and a pause was introduced between the context and the ambiguous word. Multiple access still obtained, providing further support for the claim that lexical access is not guided by syntactic context.  相似文献   
33.
How do speakers design what they say in order to communicate effectively with groups of addressees who vary in their background knowledge of the topic at hand? Prior findings indicate that when a speaker addresses a pair of listeners with discrepant knowledge, that speakers Aim Low, designing their utterances for the least knowledgeable of the two addressees. Here, we test the hypothesis that speakers will depart from an Aim Low approach in order to efficiently communicate with larger groups of interacting partners. Further, we ask whether the cognitive demands of tracking multiple conversational partners' perspectives places limitations on successful audience design. We find that speakers can successfully track information about what up to four of their partners do and do not know in conversation. When addressing groups of 3–4 addressees at once, speakers design language based on the combined knowledge of the group. These findings point to an audience design process that simultaneously represents the perspectives of multiple other individuals and combines these representations in order to design utterances that strike a balance between the different needs of the individuals within the group.  相似文献   
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
Promises     
GRANT CK 《Mind》1949,58(231):359-366
  相似文献   
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号