全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20342篇 |
免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
20347篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 3487篇 |
2017年 | 2811篇 |
2016年 | 2244篇 |
2015年 | 187篇 |
2014年 | 70篇 |
2013年 | 66篇 |
2012年 | 558篇 |
2011年 | 2381篇 |
2010年 | 2512篇 |
2009年 | 1467篇 |
2008年 | 1695篇 |
2007年 | 2167篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 208篇 |
2004年 | 160篇 |
2003年 | 109篇 |
2002年 | 58篇 |
2001年 | 29篇 |
2000年 | 50篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 9 毫秒
181.
Operant devices have been used for many years in animal behavior research, yet such devices a regenerally highly specialized and quite expensive. Although commercial models are somewhat adaptable and resilient, they are also extremely expensive and are controlled by difficult to learn proprietary software. As an alternative to commercial devices, we have designed and produced a fully functional, programmable operant device, using a PICmicro microcontroller (Microchip Technology, Inc.). The electronic operant testing apparatus (ELOPTA) is designed to deliver food when a study animal, in this case a bird, successfully depresses the correct sequence of illuminated keys. The device logs each keypress and can detect and log whenever a test animal i spositioned at the device. Data can be easily transferred to a computer and imported into any statistical analysis software. At about 3% the cost of a commercial device, ELOPTA will advance behavioral sciences, including behavioral ecology, animal learning and cognition, and ethology. 相似文献
182.
Psychological research is based in large parts on response latencies, which are often registered by keypresses on a standard computer keyboard. Recording response latencies with a standard keyboard is problematic because keypresses are buffered within the keyboard hardware before they are signaled to the computer, adding error variance to the recorded latencies. This can be circumvented by using external response pads connected to the computer's parallel port. In this article, we describe how to build inexpensive, reliable, and easy-to-use response pads with six keys from two standard computer mice that can be connected to the PC's parallel port. We also address the problem of recording data from the parallel port with different software packages under Microsoft's Windows XP. 相似文献
183.
184.
In previous work (Cesari & Newell, 2002), we used a graphical dimensional analysis to show that grip transitions obey the body-scaled relation K = lnL(o) + InM(o)h/(a + bM(h) + cL(h)), where L. and Mo are the object's length and mass, and Lh and Mh the length and mass of the grasper's hand. However, the generality of the equation was limited by the ad hoc graphical method that defined the lines for grip separation and by the assumption that these lines be negatively sloped and parallel to one another. This article reports an independent test of this relation by the geometrical and statistical categorization of body-scaled invariants for the transition of human grip configurations through support vector machines (SVMs). The SVM analysis confirmed the fit of linear, negatively sloped, and approximately parallel transition boundaries in the scaling of human grip configuration within a single hand. The SVM analysis has provided a theoretical refinement to the scaling model of human grip configurations. 相似文献
185.
Necessity and Apriority 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Gordon Prescott Barnes 《Philosophical Studies》2007,132(3):495-523
The classical view of the relationship between necessity and apriority, defended by Leibniz and Kant, is that all necessary
truths are known a priori. The classical view is now almost universally rejected, ever since Saul Kripke and Hilary Putnam
discovered that there are necessary truths that are known only a posteriori. However, in recent years a new debate has emerged
over the epistemology of these necessary a posteriori truths. According to one view – call it the neo-classical view – knowledge
of a necessary truth always depends on at least one item of a priori knowledge. According to the rival view – call it the
neoempiricist view – our knowledge of necessity is sometimes broadly empirical. In this paper I present and defend an argument
against the neo-empiricist view. I argue that knowledge of the necessity of a necessary truth could not be broadly empirical. 相似文献
186.
The present research provides evidence that people use facial prototypes when they encounter different names. In Experiment
1, participants created face exemplars for fifteen common male names, subsets of which were endorsed as good examples by a
second set of participants. These most typical faces were morphed to create face-name prototypes. In Experiment 2, participants
matched one of the names to each of the prototype faces from Experiment 1. Participants’ matching choices showed convergence
in naming the prototypes for many of the names. Experiment 3 utilized these same prototypes in a learning task designed to
investigate if the face-name associations revealed in Experiment 2 impacted the learnability of the names. Participants learned
face-name pairings that had a higher association (based on frequencies from Experiment 2) faster than pairings with a low
association. Results suggest a more direct relationship between faces and names than has been previously proposed. 相似文献
187.
Perceived heaviness has been shown to be specific to an object’s rotational inertia (I ), its resistance to rotational acceleration.
According to the kinematic specification of dynamics (KSD) principle, we hypothesized that I is optically specified by rotational
kinematics. Using virtual depictions of wielded objects, we investigated whether the visually detected rotational kinematics
of wielded objects would influence perceived heaviness in a manner consistent with the inertial model of heaviness perception.
We scaled the virtual object’s movement so that it rotated more or less than its wielded counterpart, specifying lower and
higher I, respectively. Perceived heaviness was inversely related to the rotational scaling factor, consistent with a KSD
interpretation of the inertial model. 相似文献
188.
Pezdek, Blandon-Gitlin, and Gabbay (2006) found that perceptions of the plausibility of events increase the likelihood that
imagination may induce false memories of those events. Using a survey conducted by Gallup, we asked a large sample of the
general population how plausible it would be for a person with longstanding emotional problems and a need for psychotherapy
to be a victim of childhood sexual abuse, even though the person could not remember the abuse. Only 18% indicated that it
was implausible or very implausible, whereas 67% indicated that such an occurrence was either plausible or very plausible.
Combined with Pezdek et al.’s findings, and counter to their conclusions, our findings imply that there is a substantial danger
of inducing false memories of childhood sexual abuse through imagination in psychotherapy. 相似文献
189.
Clifasefi SL Garry M Harper DN Sharman SJ Sutherland R 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2007,14(1):112-117
Can a placebo for a psychotropic drug help participants resist the misinformation effect? To answer this question, we gave participants a mixture of baking soda and water and told half of them that the mixture was a cognition-enhancing drug called R273 and told the other half that it was an inactive version of the drug. Shortly thereafter, all participants took part in a three-stage misinformation experiment. Compared with participants who were told that they had taken the placebo, the participants who were told that they had taken the drug reported improved cognitive abilities and were less susceptible to the misinformation effect. We provide sourcemonitoring and mindfulness accounts of our findings. 相似文献
190.