全文获取类型
收费全文 | 492篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
502篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 5篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 37篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 13篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 12篇 |
2006年 | 7篇 |
2005年 | 9篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有502条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
Physical size modulates the efficiency of digit comparison, depending on whether the relation of numerical magnitude and physical size is congruent or incongruent (Besner & Coltheart, Neuropsychologia, 17, 467–472, 1979), the number-size congruency effect (NSCE). In addition, Henik and Tzelgov (Memory & Cognition, 10, 389–395, 1982) first reported an NSCE for the reverse task of comparing the physical size of digits such that the numerical magnitude of digits modulated the time required to compare their physical sizes. Does the NSCE in physical comparisons simply reflect a number-mediated bias mechanism related to making decisions and selecting responses about the digit’s sizes? Alternatively, or in addition, the NSCE might indicate a true increase in the ability to discriminate small and large font sizes when these sizes are congruent with the digit’s symbolic numerical meaning, over and above response bias effects. We present a new research design that permits us to apply signal detection theory to a task that required observers to judge the physical size of digits. Our results clearly demonstrate that the NSCE cannot be reduced to mere response bias effects, and that genuine sensitivity gains for congruent number-size pairings contribute to the NSCE. 相似文献
162.
Satterthwaite TD Wolf DH Pinkham AE Ruparel K Elliott MA Valdez JN Overton E Seubert J Gur RE Gur RC Loughead J 《Brain and cognition》2011,76(3):353-363
Lesion and electrophysiological studies in animals provide evidence of opposing functions for subcortical nuclei such as the amygdala and ventral striatum, but the implications of these findings for emotion identification in humans remain poorly described. Here we report a high-resolution fMRI study in a sample of 39 healthy subjects who performed a well-characterized emotion identification task. As expected, the amygdala responded to THREAT (angry or fearful) faces more than NON-THREAT (sad or happy) faces. A functional connectivity analysis of the time series from an anatomically defined amygdala seed revealed a strong anticorrelation between the amygdala and the ventral striatum/ventral pallidum, consistent with an opposing role for these regions in during emotion identification. A second functional connectivity analysis (psychophysiological interaction) investigating relative connectivity on THREAT vs. NON-THREAT trials demonstrated that the amygdala had increased connectivity with the orbitofrontal cortex during THREAT trials, whereas the ventral striatum demonstrated increased connectivity with the posterior hippocampus on NON-THREAT trials. These results indicate that activity in the amygdala and ventral striatum may be inversely related, and that both regions may provide opposing affective bias signals during emotion identification. 相似文献
163.
Claudia C. Wolf Sebastian Ocklenburg Beyza Ören Cordula Becker Andrea Hofstätter Christa Bös Markus Popken Truls Thorstensen Onur Güntürkün 《Psychological research》2010,74(4):429-435
The stereotype of women’s limited parking skills is deeply anchored in modern culture. Although laboratory tests prove men’s
average superiority in visuospatial tasks and parking requires complex, spatial skills, underlying mechanisms remain unexplored.
Here, we investigated performance of beginners (nine women, eight men) and more experienced drivers (21 women, 27 men) at
different parking manoeuvres. Furthermore, subjects conducted the mental rotation test and self-assessed their parking skills.
We show that men park more accurately and especially faster than women. Performance is related to mental rotation skills and
self-assessment in beginners, but only to self-assessment in more experienced drivers. We assume that, due to differential
feedback, self-assessment incrementally replaces the controlling influence of mental rotation, as parking is trained with
increasing experience. Results suggest that sex differences in spatial cognition persist in real-life situations, but that
socio-psychological factors modulate the biological causes of sex differences. 相似文献
164.
Thirty previously published data sets, from seminal category learning tasks, are reanalyzed using the varying abstraction
model (VAM). Unlike a prototype-versus-exemplar analysis, which focuses on extreme levels of abstraction only, a VAM analysis
also considers the possibility of partial abstraction. Whereas most data sets support no abstraction when only the extreme
possibilities are considered, we show that evidence for abstraction can be provided using the broader view on abstraction
provided by the VAM. The present results generalize earlier demonstrations of partial abstraction (Vanpaemel & Storms, 2008),
in which only a small number of data sets was analyzed. Following the dominant modus operandi in category learning research,
Vanpaemel and Storms evaluated the models on their best fit, a practice known to ignore the complexity of the models under
consideration. In the present study, in contrast, model evaluation not only relies on the maximal likelihood, but also on
the marginal likelihood, which is sensitive to model complexity. Finally, using a large recovery study, it is demonstrated
that, across the 30 data sets, complexity differences between the models in the VAM family are small. This indicates that
a (computationally challenging) complexity-sensitive model evaluation method is uncalled for, and that the use of a (computationally
straightforward) complexity-insensitive model evaluation method is justified. 相似文献
165.
Michael P. Wolf 《Philosophical Studies》2006,130(2):351-375
Few philosophers today doubt the importance of some notion of rigid designation, as suggested by Kripke and Putnam for names
and natural kind terms. At the very least, most of us want our theories to be compatible with the most plausible elements
of that account. Anaphoric theories of reference have gained some attention lately, but little attention has been given to
how they square with rigid designation. Although the differences between anaphoric theories and many interpretations of the
New Theory of reference are substantial, I argue that rigid designation and anaphoric theories can be reconciled with one
another and in fact complement one another in important ways. 相似文献
166.
Background
Based on principles of experiential learning, psychophysical exposure (PPE) on a tightrope course offers a promising intervention in the treatment of patients with mental disorders. A PPE program conducted on a hospital tightrope course is presented as an adjunct in inpatient psychotherapy.Methods
The acceptance and effectiveness of the PPE were evaluated in a prospective controlled naturalistic study. A total of 155 patients, who were exposed at least once during their inpatient therapy, were compared with 92 control patients, who did not participate in the tightrope course. The main outcome criteria were self-reported depressive symptoms, state anxiety, locus of control, self-efficacy, and trait anxiety.Results
The PPE was highly accepted by the participants. Patients who attended the tightrope courses showed greater improvement in the outcome criteria than patients who received inpatient treatment as usual. Particularly better effects were found on the personality variables.Conclusions
Psychophysical exposure can successfully and effectively be integrated into a psychosomatic inpatient treatment program. Methodological limitations of the study, especially threats to internal validity, are discussed. 相似文献167.
Abraham W. Wolf 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》2003,33(2):129-143
John C. Norcross has made significant contributions in the areas of the transtheoretical model of change, psychotherapy integration, and empirically supported psychotherapy relationships. This interview explores the contributions of pragmatic philosophy and his personal upbringing to his work as a psychotherapy researcher and practitioner. Dr. Norcross discusses the status of the psychotherapy integration movement, the work of the Task Force on Empirically Supported Psychotherapy Relationships, and future trends in psychotherapy over the next 25 years. He emphasizes the importance of a methodological rigor that recognizes the unique characteristics of the psychotherapy relationship. He describes his current interest in the psychotherapy of psychotherapists, illustrating it with his personal challenges in balancing work and home. 相似文献
168.
Essentially all behavior is regulated by the brain in response to information received from within the body or from the environment. The tangible structures of the brain serve as devices for processing thoughts and emotions as well as information. Stored among the interacting neural structures are memories of past experiences and responses to them. These intangibles participate in determining the decisions made and the actions performed by the brain's structures. There are valuable studies of the clinical and neurological effects of environmental stimuli, but we need to learn more about the processes that lead to these effects. More definitive correlations could be made between environmental stimuli and the neurological pathways they create by studying individual's real life experiences rather than laboratory simulations alone. 相似文献
169.
Abraham W. Wolf Daniel S.P. Schubert Marian Patterson Thomas Grande Linda Pendleton 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(3-4):747-755
We evaluated the hypothesis that characterological factors, particularly antisocial personality, influence scores on the MacAndrew Alcoholism Scale (MAC). Using the Washington University research criteria to establish diagnoses, the following subgroups were defined: subjects who received no psychiatric diagnoses; subjects who received diagnoses other than alcoholism, drug dependence, or antisocial personality; and subjects who received a diagnosis of alcoholism, drug dependence, and/or antisocial personality, alone or in combination with other psychiatric diagnoses. The highest MAC scores were obtained by subjects diagnosed as having antisocial personality without alcoholism or drug dependence and subjects diagnosed with antisocial personality, drug dependence, and alcoholism. Alcoholics without antisocial personality or drug dependence, and subjects with other psychiatric diagnoses scored lowest. Although higher MAC scores were obtained by males, MAC scores greater than 24 correctly classified the greatest number of males and females as alcoholic, drug dependent and/or antisocial. These findings are interpreted as consistent with MacAndrew's reinterpretation of the scale as assessing a dimension of personality, rather than solely a tendency to addiction. 相似文献
170.
Lipopolysaccharide-induced experimental immune activation does not impair memory functions in humans
Jan-Sebastian Grigoleit J. Reiner Oberbeck Philipp Lichte Philipp Kobbe Oliver T. Wolf Thomas Montag Adriana del Rey Elke R. Gizewski Harald Engler Manfred Schedlowski 《Neurobiology of learning and memory》2010,94(4):561-567
Reconsolidation studies have led to the hypothesis that memory, when labile, would be modified in order to incorporate new information. This view has reinstated original propositions suggesting that short-term memory provides the organism with an opportunity to evaluate and rearrange information before storing it, since it is concurrent with the labile state of consolidation. The Chasmagnathus associative memory model is used here to test whether during consolidation it is possible to change some attribute of recently acquired memories. In addition, it is tested whether these changes in behavioral memory features can be explained as modifications on the consolidating memory trace or as a consequence of a new memory trace. We show that short-term memory is, unlike long-term memory, not context specific. During this short period after learning, behavioral memory can be updated in order to incorporate new contextual information. We found that, during this period, the cycloheximide retrograde amnesic effect can be reverted by a single trial in a new context. Finally, by means of memory sensitivity to cycloheximide during consolidation and reconsolidation, we show that the learning of a new context (CS) during this short-term memory period builds up a new memory trace that sustains the behavioral memory update. 相似文献