首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   491篇
  免费   27篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   8篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   11篇
  2019年   23篇
  2018年   29篇
  2017年   20篇
  2016年   33篇
  2015年   19篇
  2014年   17篇
  2013年   50篇
  2012年   32篇
  2011年   37篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   18篇
  2008年   18篇
  2007年   19篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   12篇
  2004年   19篇
  2003年   10篇
  2002年   14篇
  2001年   9篇
  2000年   9篇
  1999年   8篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   3篇
  1994年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1990年   4篇
  1989年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   2篇
  1986年   1篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   3篇
  1973年   1篇
排序方式: 共有518条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
511.
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review - Exposure to a natural disaster can have a myriad of significant and adverse psychological consequences. Children have been identified as a particularly...  相似文献   
512.
Young children exhibit a video deficit for spatial recall, learning less from on-screen than in-person demonstrations. Some theoretical accounts emphasize memory constraints (e.g., insufficient retrieval cues, competition between memory representations). Such accounts imply memory representations are graded, yet video deficit studies measuring spatial recall operationalize memory retrieval as dichotomous (success or failure). The current study tested a graded-representation account using a spatial recall task with a continuous search space (i.e., sandbox) rather than discrete locations. With this more sensitive task, a protracted video deficit for spatial recall was found in children 4–5 years old (n = 51). This may be due to weaker memory representations in the screen condition, evidenced by higher variability and greater perseverative bias. In general, perseverative bias decreased with repeated trials. The discussion considers how the results support a graded-representation account, potentially explaining why children might exhibit a video deficit in some tasks but not others.

Research Highlights

  • The task used a continuous search space (sandbox), making it more difficult and sensitive than spatial recall tasks used in prior video deficit research.
  • Spatial recall among 4- and 5-year-old children was more variable after watching hiding events on screen via live video feed than through a window.
  • Children's spatial recall from screens was more susceptible to proactive interference, evidenced by more perseverative bias in an A-not-B design.
  • The results demonstrate memory representations blend experiences that accumulate over time and explain why the video deficit may be protracted for more difficult tasks.
  相似文献   
513.
514.
515.
Feminists have argued that traditional gender norms can obstruct women's sexual well-being ( Amaro, 1995 ; Morokoff, 2000 ; Tolman, 2006 ; Walker, 1997 ). Therefore, we expected feminist ideology, by virtue of this critique, to be associated with women's sexual subjectivity and sexual well-being. To test this model, we analyzed data from a survey of college-age women ( N = 424) using structural equation modeling. As hypothesized, feminist ideology was indirectly related to condom-use self-efficacy and sexual satisfaction via sexual subjectivity, and sexual motivation was directly related to sexual satisfaction. In an alternative model, feminist ideology was directly related to sexual motivation. This research indicates that feminist ideology may play a role in the promotion of women's sexual well-being.  相似文献   
516.
517.
518.
Animals are important stimuli for humans, and for children in particular. In three experiments, we explored children's affinity for animals. In Experiment 1, 11‐ to 40‐month‐old children were presented with a free‐play session in which they were encouraged to interact with several interesting toys and two live animals – a fish and a hamster. Experiment 2 used the same methodology with 18‐ to 36‐month‐old children and two additional animals – a snake and a spider – to examine whether children's behaviours would differ for benign and potentially threatening animals. Finally, in Experiment 3, a more controlled paired‐preference paradigm was employed to assess 18‐ to 33‐month‐old children's interactions with three live animals – a fish, hamster, and gecko – versus three physically similar toy animals. Across all three experiments, children interacted with the animals more often than with the toys. Further, they behaved differently towards the animals than the toys, talking about the animals more than the toys and asking more questions about them. The parents of the children also spent more time interacting with the animals, directing their children's attention more towards the animals than the toys. This research supports the idea that humans have an affinity for animals that draws their attention to animals, even when attractive toys are present.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号