全文获取类型
收费全文 | 288篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
300篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 4篇 |
2021年 | 8篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 21篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 32篇 |
2012年 | 25篇 |
2011年 | 23篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 14篇 |
2008年 | 14篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 5篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 6篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有300条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Research suggests that exposure to sociocultural norms for idealized appearance can reduce both women's and men's body satisfaction. Despite comparable effects for both genders in the lab, in the “real-world” women's body satisfaction is chronically lower than men's. Real-world gender differences may arise from discrepancies in men's and women's everyday exposure to norms. Across eight studies using a variety of content analysis, survey, and experimental methods, we examine differences in sociocultural norms for ideal appearance pertaining to women and men in “daily life” contexts. We demonstrate that appearance norms encountered by women in daily life are more rigid, homogenous and pervasive than those for men, and that more messages implying the attainability of the ideal appearance are directed at women. Finally, experimental results show that homogeneous, rigid norms (like those typically encountered by women) are more harmful to body image than heterogeneous, flexible norms (like those typically encountered by men). 相似文献
73.
The dramatic rise in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) has resulted in a significant explosion in visibility
and a substantial increase in the understanding of this complex group of disorders. Over the last decade, what was once defined
only as “autism” has become a spectrum of neurodevelopmental disorders, with variable presentations and levels of impairment
that requires an equally broad continuum of care. A wealth of research has not discovered a single “cause” for ASDs, but rather
has found a range of genetic/genomic variations that likely play a significant role in the etiology of these disorders. Routine
screening for autism is becoming more common, allowing for more timely diagnosis, and hopefully for earlier entry into appropriate
and effective treatment. Although there remains no single ‘‘cure’’, there are treatments available that can improve overall
functioning and decrease problematic or interfering symptoms across the full spectrum of this disorder. The wide spectrum
of ASD presentations complicates treatment planning, with overall ASD severity a factor in determining type, intensity, and
duration of interventions and services. This article presents a two-dimensional model of “ASD severity” that considers both
the level of specific ASD symptoms/deficits and the level of cognitive resources/limitations as a framework for understanding
needs, challenges, and potentially effective interventions for individuals across the ASD spectrum. 相似文献
74.
Merz EL Malcarne VL Roesch SC Riley N Sadler GR 《Cultural diversity & ethnic minority psychology》2011,17(3):309-316
Depression is a significant problem for ethnic minorities that remains understudied partly due to a lack of strong measures with established psychometric properties. One screening tool, the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), which was developed for use in primary care has also gained popularity in research settings. The reliability and validity of the PHQ-9 has been well established among predominantly Caucasian samples, in addition to many minority groups. However, there is little evidence regarding its utility among Hispanic Americans, a large and growing cultural group in the United States. In this study, we investigated the reliability and structural validity of the PHQ-9 in Hispanic American women. A community sample of 479 Latina women from southern California completed the PHQ-9 in their preferred language of English or Spanish. Cronbach's alphas suggested that there was good internal consistency for both the English- and Spanish-language versions. Structural validity was investigated using multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. Results support a similar one-factor structure with equivalent response patterns and variances among English- and Spanish-speaking Latinas. These results suggest that the PHQ-9 can be used with confidence in both English and Spanish versions to screen Latinas for depression. 相似文献
75.
76.
Although lexicosemantic deficits are not typically seen in older adults, some studies indicate that age-related changes in semantic processing may occur. We had groups of older and younger adults perform speeded lexical decision on mass (e.g., honey), count (e.g., car), and dual nouns, which may be either mass or count (e.g., lamb). Singular dual nouns engendered significantly faster response times in older adults than mass and count nouns, whereas younger adults manifested similar response times to count and dual nouns. These results point toward a three-way distinction in the lexicon between mass, count, and dual nouns. Older adults appear to treat a larger set of nouns as dual than do younger adults. This may be due to awareness of the mass/count ambiguity present in a greater number of lexical items, as a result of their greater linguistic experience. Alternatively, in order to conserve processing resources, older adults may not activate mass/count information when recognizing a dual noun unless a mass or count reading is forced by context. 相似文献
77.
78.
Throughout an investigation, pieces of evidence are likely to contaminate one another, yet at trial jurors are expected to treat pieces of evidence as if they are independent. Are jurors able to understand potential evidence contamination? The present study showed mock jurors a videotaped trial simulation. Participants were randomly assigned to hear testimony regarding one piece of evidence, two pieces of independent evidence, or two pieces of interdependent evidence. The study tested the hypothesis that jurors who hear evidence that is interdependent will be just as likely to find the defendant guilty as jurors who hear about two pieces of independent evidence. When an eyewitness's identification was the uncontaminated piece of evidence, our hypothesis was supported. However, when the confession was the uncontaminated piece of evidence, jurors seemed to understand that one piece of evidence had been influenced by another and adjusted their beliefs about the defendant's guilt accordingly. This study supports the conclusion that jurors can sometimes identify and correct for evidence contamination in their perceptions of a defendant's guilt. Implications for reform support are discussed. 相似文献
79.
Lana Vedelago Michael Amlung Vanessa Morris Tashia Petker Iris Balodis Kaitlyn McLachlan Mini Mamak Heather Moulden Gary Chaimowitz James MacKillop 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2019,37(4):435-451
Deficits in impulse control have been linked to criminal offending, risk of recidivism, and other maladaptive behaviours relevant to the criminal justice system (e.g. substance use). Impulse control can be conceptualized as encompassing the broad domains of response inhibition and impulsive/risky decision‐making. Advancements in technology have led to the development of computerized behavioural measures to assess performance in these domains, such as go/no‐go and delay discounting tasks. Despite a relatively large literature examining these tasks in offenders, findings are not universally consistent. This systematic review aims to synthesize the literature using computerized neurocognitive tasks to assess two domains of impulse control in offenders: response inhibition and impulsive/risky decision‐making. The review included 28 studies from diverse geographic locations, settings, and offender populations. The results largely support the general conclusion that offenders exhibit deficits in impulse control compared with non‐offenders, with studies of response inhibition more consistently reporting differences than studies using impulsive and risky decision‐making tasks. Findings are discussed in the context of contemporary neuroimaging research emphasizing dysfunction in prefrontal cortex as a key contributor to impulse control deficits in offenders. 相似文献
80.
A 9-year-old clinic-referred boy, his mother, and his teacher were observed in 38 home and 38 school sessions on the same days. Categories of the boy's oppositional behavior and the inappropriate social attention of his mother and teacher were graphed to visually inspect changes during baseline, a parent-training phase, a follow-up phase, and a final parent-training booster phase. Parent-training phases produced reductions in the mother's inappropriate attention and in the boy's oppositional behavior, whereas the follow-up and baseline phases were associated with higher rates of these categories. Generalization occurred in the school across these home phases, as seen in the increase in rates of the boy's problem behavior, despite the lack of change in his teacher's attention. Correlational analyses of proportion scores reflecting the boy's home-school oppositional behavior and mother-teacher social attention suggested his responsiveness to relative changes in adult social contingencies across settings. 相似文献