全文获取类型
收费全文 | 25464篇 |
免费 | 445篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
25912篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 252篇 |
2019年 | 261篇 |
2018年 | 384篇 |
2017年 | 436篇 |
2016年 | 473篇 |
2015年 | 286篇 |
2014年 | 388篇 |
2013年 | 1940篇 |
2012年 | 739篇 |
2011年 | 795篇 |
2010年 | 491篇 |
2009年 | 454篇 |
2008年 | 681篇 |
2007年 | 651篇 |
2006年 | 596篇 |
2005年 | 529篇 |
2004年 | 530篇 |
2003年 | 486篇 |
2002年 | 513篇 |
2001年 | 775篇 |
2000年 | 793篇 |
1999年 | 583篇 |
1998年 | 289篇 |
1997年 | 247篇 |
1996年 | 232篇 |
1992年 | 478篇 |
1991年 | 432篇 |
1990年 | 472篇 |
1989年 | 428篇 |
1988年 | 423篇 |
1987年 | 411篇 |
1986年 | 419篇 |
1985年 | 444篇 |
1984年 | 352篇 |
1983年 | 313篇 |
1982年 | 256篇 |
1981年 | 229篇 |
1979年 | 406篇 |
1978年 | 294篇 |
1977年 | 271篇 |
1975年 | 355篇 |
1974年 | 391篇 |
1973年 | 363篇 |
1972年 | 339篇 |
1971年 | 296篇 |
1970年 | 270篇 |
1969年 | 328篇 |
1968年 | 369篇 |
1967年 | 327篇 |
1966年 | 329篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
831.
Damian Scarf Michael C. Corballis Onur Güntürkün Michael Colombo 《Animal cognition》2017,20(5):999-1002
Many children pass through a mirror stage in reading, where they write individual letters or digits in mirror and find it difficult to correctly utilize letters that are mirror images of one another (e.g., b and d). This phenomenon is thought to reflect the fact that the brain does not naturally discriminate left from right. Indeed, it has been argued that reading acquisition involves the inhibition of this default process. In the current study, we tested the ability of literate pigeons, which had learned to discriminate between 30 and 62 words from 7832 nonwords, to discriminate between words and their mirror counterparts. Subjects were sensitive to the left–right orientation of the individual letters, but not the order of letters within a word. This finding may reflect the fact that, in the absence of human-unique top-down processes, the inhibition of mirror generalization may be limited. 相似文献
832.
833.
834.
Iris Schutte J. Leon Kenemans Dennis J. L. G. Schutter 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2017,17(4):754-763
Prior research has shown that the ratio between resting-state theta (4–7 Hz)-beta (13–30 Hz) oscillations in the electroencephalogram (EEG) is associated with reward- and punishment-related feedback learning and risky decision making. However, it remains unclear whether the theta/beta EEG ratio is also an electrophysiological index for poorer behavioral adaptation when reward and punishment contingencies change over time. The aim of the present study was to investigate whether resting-state theta (4–7 Hz)-beta (13–30 Hz) EEG ratio correlated with reversal learning. A 4-min resting-state EEG was recorded and a gambling task with changing reward-punishment contingencies was administered in 128 healthy volunteers. Results showed an inverse relationship between theta/beta EEG ratio and reversal learning. Our findings replicate and extend previous findings by showing that higher midfrontal theta/beta EEG ratios are associated with poorer reversal learning and behavioral adaptive responses under changing environmental demands. 相似文献
835.
Xin Hou Timothy A. Allen Dongtao Wei Hui Huang Kangcheng Wang Colin G. DeYoung Jiang Qiu 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2017,17(5):1018-1027
Individual differences in the personality trait Agreeableness underlie humans’ ability to interpret social cues and coordinate effectively with others. However, previous investigations of the neural basis of Agreeableness have yielded largely inconsistent results. Recent evidence has demonstrated that Agreeableness can be divided into two, correlated subdimensions. Compassion reflects tendencies toward empathy, sympathy, and concern for others, while Politeness reflects tendencies toward compliance and refraining from aggression and exploitation. The present study seeks to clarify the neural substrates of Agreeableness by examining whether structural differences in the brain show distinct associations with Compassion and Politeness. Results of a meta-analysis of fMRI studies examining empathy were used to generate hypotheses about the brain regions and networks that underlie trait Compassion. Results of a large-scale structural neuroimaging investigation (N = 275) were largely consistent with the meta-analysis: Compassion was positively correlated with gray matter volume in the bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and anterior insula (AI). Further, these differences appear to be associated with Compassion specifically, as opposed to Politeness, suggesting that these two traits have at least partially distinct neuroanatomical substrates. 相似文献
836.
Mario Gómez‐Torrente 《No?s (Detroit, Mich.)》2017,51(2):429-435
Nat Hansen builds a new argument for subjectivism about the semantics of color language, based on a potential kind of intersubjective disagreements about comparative color statements. In reply, I note that the disagreements of this kind are merely hypothetical, probably few if actual, and not evidently relevant as test cases for a semantic theory. Furthermore, even if they turned out to be actual and semantically relevant, they would be intuitively unusable by the subjectivist. 相似文献
837.
Anthony C. Hood Kevin S. Cruz Daniel G. Bachrach 《Journal of business and psychology》2017,32(1):73-86
Drawing on conservation of resources theory, multiplex social networks research, and the emerging conflict involvement perspective, the purpose of this study is to develop and test a multiplex view of conflict that explicitly accounts for the nature of the social relationships between those involved in intrateam conflict and how these multiplex relationships differentially impact team performance. Data were collected from 120 teams engaged in a 4-month business simulation. Relationship conflicts occurring among team members who are friends have a negative impact on team performance, whereas those occurring between non-friends have a positive impact on team performance. Although we also find non-friend task conflicts to be beneficial for team performance, friend task conflicts have no impact on team performance. This study highlights the dark side of workplace friendships and admonishes managers to pay close attention not only to conflicts among employees, but also to the relational closeness of those involved in conflict. The current study provides empirical support for the emerging conflict involvement perspective by explicitly assessing the number of individuals involved in conflict as well as the type of relationships between them. We also extend research on multiplex relationships from the individual to the team level of analysis. Finally, we respond to calls for studies of multiplexity that include both positive and negative relationships. 相似文献
838.
Nanette L. Yragui Caitlin A. Demsky Leslie B. Hammer Sarah Van Dyck Moni B. Neradilek 《Journal of business and psychology》2017,32(2):179-196
Purpose
The present study examined the moderating effects of family-supportive supervisor behaviors (FSSB) on the relationship between two types of workplace aggression (i.e., patient-initiated physical aggression and coworker-initiated psychological aggression) and employee well-being and work outcomes.Methodology
Data were obtained from a field sample of 417 healthcare workers in two psychiatric hospitals. Hypotheses were tested using moderated multiple regression analyses.Findings
Psychiatric care providers’ perceptions of FSSB moderated the relationship between patient-initiated physical aggression and physical symptoms, exhaustion and cynicism. In addition, FSSB moderated the relationship between coworker-initiated psychological aggression and physical symptoms and turnover intentions.Implications
Based on our findings, family-supportive supervision is a plausible boundary condition for the relationship between workplace aggression and well-being and work outcomes. This study suggests that, in addition to directly addressing aggression prevention and reduction, family-supportive supervision is a trainable resource that healthcare organizations should facilitate to improve employee work and well-being in settings with high workplace aggression.Originality
This is the first study to examine the role of FSSB in influencing the relationship between two forms of workplace aggression: patient-initiated physical and coworker-initiated psychological aggression and employee outcomes.839.
Garett N. Howardson Michael N. Karim Ryan G. Horn 《Journal of business and psychology》2017,32(3):317-334
Purpose
This research advances understanding of empirical time modeling techniques in self-regulated learning research. We intuitively explain several such methods by situating their use in the extant literature. Further, we note key statistical and inferential assumptions of each method while making clear the inferential consequences of inattention to such assumptions.Design/Methodology/Approach
Using a population model derived from a recent large-scale review of the training and work learning literature, we employ a Monte Carlo simulation fitting six variations of linear mixed models, seven variations of latent common factor models, and a single latent change score model to 1500 simulated datasets.Findings
The latent change score model outperformed all six of the linear mixed models and all seven of the latent common factor models with respect to (1) estimation precision of the average learner improvement, (2) correctly rejecting a false null hypothesis about such average improvement, and (3) correctly failing to reject true null hypothesis about between-learner differences (i.e., random slopes) in average improvement.Implications
The latent change score model is a more flexible method of modeling time in self-regulated learning research, particularly for learner processes consistent with twenty-first-century workplaces. Consequently, defaulting to linear mixed or latent common factor modeling methods may have adverse inferential consequences for better understanding self-regulated learning in twenty-first-century work.Originality/Value
Ours is the first study to critically, rigorously, and empirically evaluate self-regulated learning modeling methods and to provide a more flexible alternative consistent with modern self-regulated learning knowledge.840.
Juhani Järvikivi Roger P. G. van Gompel Jukka Hyönä 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2017,46(3):525-550
Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigating pronoun resolution in Finnish examined the time course of implicit causality information relative to both grammatical role and order-of-mention information. Experiment 1 showed an effect of implicit causality that appeared at the same time as the first-mention preference. Furthermore, when we counterbalanced the semantic roles of the verbs, we found no effect of grammatical role, suggesting the standard observed subject preference has a large semantic component. Experiment 2 showed that both the personal pronoun hän and the demonstrative tämä preferred the antecedent consistent with the implicit causality bias; tämä was not interpreted as referring to the semantically non-prominent entity. In contrast, structural prominence affected hän and tämä differently: we found a first-mention preference for hän, but a second-mention preference for tämä. The results suggest that semantic implicit causality information has an immediate effect on pronoun resolution and its use is not delayed relative to order-of-mention information. Furthermore, they show that order-of-mention differentially affects different types of anaphoric expressions, but semantic information has the same effect. 相似文献