全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2955篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
2989篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 87篇 |
2023年 | 26篇 |
2022年 | 29篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 193篇 |
2019年 | 251篇 |
2018年 | 80篇 |
2017年 | 90篇 |
2016年 | 273篇 |
2015年 | 245篇 |
2014年 | 79篇 |
2013年 | 239篇 |
2012年 | 104篇 |
2011年 | 79篇 |
2010年 | 68篇 |
2009年 | 61篇 |
2008年 | 79篇 |
2007年 | 84篇 |
2006年 | 76篇 |
2005年 | 56篇 |
2004年 | 69篇 |
2003年 | 68篇 |
2002年 | 70篇 |
2001年 | 34篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 33篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 23篇 |
1996年 | 17篇 |
1995年 | 16篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 24篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 12篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1970年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2989条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
Simon Davis 《Sex roles》1990,23(1-2):43-50
A study was made of 328 personal advertisements sampled from a major daily Canadian newspaper. It was found that gender differences for desired companion attributes were consistent with traditional sex role stereotypes. Relative to the opposite sex, women emphasized employment, financial, and intellectual status, as well as commitment, while men emphasized physical characteristics. Physical characteristics were the most desired, regardless of sex. Secondary findings were that, for this sample, considerably more men than women placed ads, and that the mean age for both sexes was relatively high. The main findings were similar to those from earlier studies.The author was formerly a psychiatric social worker employed by the Greater Vancouver Mental Health Service in Vancouver, B.C., Canada. He is presently a doctoral candidate at Simon Fraser University in Vancouver. The author would like to thank Dr. Ted Palys for his comments. 相似文献
12.
Valerie J. Steffen Louis Sternberg Lisa A. Teegarden Kristen Shepherd 《Journal of applied social psychology》1994,24(10):897-925
Many behaviors are performed less frequently than intended because they require knowledge and skill to overcome behavioral barriers. This experiment tested effects of two factors, direct experience and message frame, that were hypothesized to affect men's intention to perform the testicle self-exam for cancer (TSE) and their actual exam performance, in part by affecting their knowledge and beliefs about overcoming TSE performance barriers. Men's experience performing the TSE on a life-like model and the frame (negative, positive, or neutral) of the recommendation promoting the exam were manipulated factorially. Consistent with prediction, men who practiced (vs. did not practice) the self-exam: (a) endorsed stronger beliefs about behavior-specific knowledge (e.g., ability to imagine a lump), (b) held more positive intention, and (c) translated that intention more consistently into self-reported action, in part because their behavior-specific knowledge increased their tendency to act on their intention. Consistent with action phase theory (Gollwitzer, 1990), intention related more strongly to behavior-specific beliefs than to general, long-term beliefs, and more strongly than attitude related to behavior-specific beliefs. These findings have practical and theoretical implications for promoting healthful and other intended behaviors. 相似文献
13.
A seminal attentional model of time estimation predicts that subjective duration will be positively correlated to the amount of attention given to temporal processing. This prediction holds under prospective conditions, in which one is forewarned that judgments of time will be asked, in contrast to retrospective conditions, in which such judgments are required after the relevant period without any prior warning. In three experiments, an attention-sharing method was used. Subjects were asked to control the amount of attention that they devoted to one or the other component of a dual-task paradigm. The first experiment involved word categorization and reproduction of duration. The following experiments, based on signal detection theory, required discrimination of both the duration and the intensity of a single stimulus, in the visual (Experiment 2) or the auditory (Experiment 3) modality. The results indicate that when the attention is directly controlled by the subject, the subjective duration shortens as the amount of attention devoted to the temporal task diminishes. The implications of these results for the possible existence of an internal timer are considered. 相似文献
14.
Clinical psychologists' and nonpsychiatric physicians' attitudes and behaviors in sexual and confidentiality boundary violations were examined. The 171 participants' responses were analyzed by profession, sex, and status (student, resident, professional) on semantic differential, boundary violation vignettes, and a version of Pope, Tabachnick, and Keith-Spiegel's (1987) ethical scale. Psychologists rated sexual boundary violation as more unethical than did physicians (p less than .001). Rationale (p less than .01) and timing (p less than .0001) influenced ratings. Psychologists reported fewer sexualized behaviors than physicians (p less than .05). Professional experience (p less than .01) and sex (p less than .05) were associated with confidence-violating behavior. Overall, 78% of the sample reported attitudes or behaviors associated with boundary violations. The behavior violations were correlated (r = .49). Actual violators rated vignette violators more leniently than did nonviolators (p less than .01). 相似文献
15.
REM motivation induced by brief REM deprivation: the influence of cognition, gender, and personality. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
D B Cohen M J McGarth L W Bell M J Hanlon N Simon 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1978,36(7):741-751
Prominence of the brain's right-hemisphere information processing and intensity of dream experience are two theoretically related constructs that have been proposed as important psychological aspects of REM sleep. Either view is consistent with the prediction that the effect of REM deprivation will depend in part on the nature of cognitive activity that is initiated at the onset of each REM period and that "substitutes" for the interrupted REM process. In the present study, the effect of REM deprivation was more striking for female subjects given a digits task than for female subjects given a fantasy-reporting task during awakening used to induce REM deprivation for the first 6 hours of the night. High neuroticism appeared to exaggerate the effect. No corresponding pattern was observed for the male subjects. These preliminary finding may be exaggerated by cognitive activity that is functionally incongruent with those processes. The results also raise interesting questions about individual differences. 相似文献
16.
Beauty Is Only "Name" Deep: The Effect of First-Name On Ratings of Physical Attraction 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S. Gary Garwood Lewis Cox Valerie Kaplan Neal Wasserman Jefferson L. Sulzer 《Journal of applied social psychology》1980,10(5):431-435
College students ( n = 197) voted to select a beauty queen from among six photographs equivalent in physical attractiveness, as determined by rankings of 35 students from a separate university. Half the photographs bore a desirable first-name and half were assigned an undesirable first-name. As predicted, firstname had a significant effect on physical attraction: Girls with desirable firstnames received 158 votes to 39 for those with undesirable names. These data were discussed in terms of an additive, rather than an interactive, effect, due to the nature of the task. The strength of these findings was related to the study's ecological validity. 相似文献
17.
Valerie Tate Angel C.S.W. Carole Katz C.S.W. Arlene Litwack M.S. Mary Maffia Ph.D. Robert A. Mednick Ph.D. Richard Mingoia C.S.W. Rene Rocha Ph.D. 《Group》1983,7(3):55-61
This paper reflects the working through of a group's fantasy that the deceased group leader wished the group to continue. As in any defense, the fantasy protected members from the pain of the finality of the leader's death. This article reveals the subjective experience concerning the effect of the leader's death on the group and members' intrapsychic struggle to separate and individuate from the deceased leader, while attending to and striving toward the group's continuation. The group's painful and growing individuation enabled the group to follow its own tasks without the spector of the deceased leader. 相似文献
18.
The relative contribution of the instructions and the music to the effects of the Musical Mood Induction Procedure (MMIP) were investigated. Using an experimental procedure which minimised covert experimenter bias, subjects performed under both elation and depression mood inductions in one of four conditions: music present or absent by mood change instructions present or absent, using a crossover design. Results indicated that instructions were both necessary and sufficient to produce change on mood sensitive measures. Mood change effects were reduced by controlling for the effect of subjects who responded so as not to ‘louse-up’ the experiment. The MMIP seems to be much less musical than the name implies. Further research is needed to determine which aspects of the instructions contribute most to the changes on mood sensitive measures. 相似文献
19.
Valerie Gray Hardcastle 《Synthese》1991,86(2):209-228
I wish to thank Elisabeth Lloyd, Samuel Mitchell, Greg Ray, Piers Rollins and two anonymous readers for their insightful criticisms and comments. 相似文献
20.
Charles M. Gibbs Valerie Cool Tamio Land E. James Kehoe I. Gormezano 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1991,26(4):282-295
Second-order conditioning of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response (NMR) was investigated when second-order trials (CS1-CS2) were intermixed with first-order trials (CS2-US) from the outset of training. Experiment 1 showed that CR acquisition to CS1 was inversely related to the CS1-CS2 interval but nevertheless extended to an interval of 8,400 ms. Experiment 2 revealed that CR acquisition of CS1 was an inverted-U function of the number of CS1-CS2 trials relative to a fixed number of CS2-US trials. Experiment 3 directly contrasted second-order conditioning with a reinforced serial compound procedure (CS1-CS2-US) and a mixed procedure in which second-order trials were intermixed with the reinforced serial compound. Second-order conditioning was about half the strength of either the reinforced serial compound or the mixed procedure, which were similar. The present results are discussed with respect to the relative strength of excitatory and inhibitory processes in second-order conditioning. 相似文献