首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   63899篇
  免费   2578篇
  国内免费   20篇
  2020年   615篇
  2019年   810篇
  2018年   1071篇
  2017年   1113篇
  2016年   1173篇
  2015年   872篇
  2014年   1017篇
  2013年   4768篇
  2012年   1888篇
  2011年   2074篇
  2010年   1274篇
  2009年   1267篇
  2008年   1786篇
  2007年   1815篇
  2006年   1652篇
  2005年   1456篇
  2004年   1353篇
  2003年   1264篇
  2002年   1391篇
  2001年   2103篇
  2000年   2082篇
  1999年   1564篇
  1998年   723篇
  1997年   643篇
  1996年   647篇
  1993年   588篇
  1992年   1315篇
  1991年   1225篇
  1990年   1200篇
  1989年   1093篇
  1988年   1078篇
  1987年   1035篇
  1986年   1117篇
  1985年   1128篇
  1984年   945篇
  1983年   854篇
  1982年   601篇
  1981年   605篇
  1979年   1015篇
  1978年   714篇
  1975年   814篇
  1974年   897篇
  1973年   978篇
  1972年   820篇
  1971年   780篇
  1970年   699篇
  1969年   724篇
  1968年   907篇
  1967年   843篇
  1966年   686篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
Memory bias in clinical anxiety   总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6  
  相似文献   
922.
Hand movements: a window into haptic object recognition   总被引:15,自引:0,他引:15  
  相似文献   
923.
924.
Work values and key influencers of a sample of black male and female adolescents were examined. Results indicated that boys and girls valued both the intrinsic and extrinsic rewards of work; however, girls reported slighter stronger extrinsic values than did boys. In addition, the sexes reported differences in the importance of specific work values such as "making lots of money," and "doing important things." When naming a key influencer, respondents tended to cite a same-sex and race individual. Sex of one's key influencer was related to certain work values, with subjects reporting a male key influencer valuing "trying out one's own ideas" and "having a secure future" more than those reporting a female key influencer. The interaction of sex of subject and sex of key influencer was significant on one of the work value outcomes. Implications of these findings are considered.  相似文献   
925.
926.
927.
Age differences in memory for item and source information   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
928.
This study examined the relationships between metamemory and strategic behavior in impulsive and reflective children. One hundred thirty children from the fourth, fifth, and sixth grades participated. Seventy-seven of these children had been tested 3 years earlier on metamemory and cognitive tempo tasks. At pretraining, children were assessed on metamemory, cognitive tempo, summarization skills, and teacher ratings of impulsive behavior in the classroom. Next, children in three experimental groups received prose summarization instructions, summarization instructions in conjunction with metacognitive training about the importance of a reflective approach to learning, or no instructions. Following training, children were again measured on tempo, summarization skills, and teacher ratings of impulsivity. Analyses of strategy maintenance data indicated superior performance for children who had received both summarization and metacognitive training. Causal modeling analyses showed that early metamemory was an antecedent of later strategy acquisition. The dual role of metacognition as a precursor of later strategy acquisition and controller of lower level strategies was highlighted.  相似文献   
929.
The present study investigated effects of age and instructions on temporal regulations of behavior in children. In the first experiment 4 1/2-year-old and 7-year-old subjects were trained with a DRL (differential reinforcement of low rates) 5-s and a DRL 10-s schedule. Results demonstrate that age and timing performance are related. Seven-year-olds are more efficient than the 4 1/2-year-olds. A striking decline in the 4 1/2-year-old children's capacity to space responses was observed in the DRL 10-s schedule as compared to the DRL 5-s schedule. Analysis of individual performances suggests that the evolution of DRL performance between 4 and 7 years of age depends not only on the development of the capacity to delay responding but also on the acquisition of the ability to represent the reinforcement contingencies, that is, the temporal parameters of the task to oneself. In order to test this hypothesis a second experiment was conducted where instructions to wait between operant responses were given to a group of 4 1/2-year-old subjects at the beginning of a DRL 5-s and a DRL 10-s schedule. The results show that these instructions enhance DRL performance. By directing the 4 1/2-year-old subjects' attention to the temporal requirements of the task, instructions led to efficient performance and accurate timing of responses to the DRL schedule.  相似文献   
930.
We have identified a fundamental property of human motor behavior as a tight coupling of the curvature-speed relationship in the reaching movements of 5- to 9-month-old infants. This relationship termed a movement unit, occurs regardless of the distance of duration of the reach and in spite of the developmental change that occurs in grasping during this period. Movement unit durations are tightly clustered around 200 ms regardless of overall duration or distance or the position of the unit in the reach. The curvature-speed coupling has been identified by others in adult reaching and handwriting. Models of biological motor control must account for this invariant relationship.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号