全文获取类型
收费全文 | 9606篇 |
免费 | 185篇 |
专业分类
9791篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 79篇 |
2022年 | 72篇 |
2021年 | 94篇 |
2020年 | 161篇 |
2019年 | 171篇 |
2018年 | 264篇 |
2017年 | 268篇 |
2016年 | 291篇 |
2015年 | 224篇 |
2014年 | 263篇 |
2013年 | 978篇 |
2012年 | 392篇 |
2011年 | 394篇 |
2010年 | 246篇 |
2009年 | 241篇 |
2008年 | 298篇 |
2007年 | 306篇 |
2006年 | 261篇 |
2005年 | 193篇 |
2004年 | 204篇 |
2003年 | 208篇 |
2002年 | 189篇 |
2001年 | 244篇 |
2000年 | 265篇 |
1999年 | 187篇 |
1998年 | 81篇 |
1997年 | 67篇 |
1996年 | 70篇 |
1993年 | 71篇 |
1992年 | 128篇 |
1991年 | 127篇 |
1990年 | 116篇 |
1989年 | 104篇 |
1988年 | 96篇 |
1987年 | 102篇 |
1986年 | 105篇 |
1985年 | 105篇 |
1984年 | 102篇 |
1983年 | 81篇 |
1979年 | 119篇 |
1976年 | 67篇 |
1975年 | 86篇 |
1974年 | 111篇 |
1973年 | 96篇 |
1972年 | 88篇 |
1971年 | 89篇 |
1970年 | 89篇 |
1969年 | 88篇 |
1968年 | 79篇 |
1967年 | 94篇 |
排序方式: 共有9791条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
811.
Age-related changes in rate of visual information processing 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
V Di Lollo J L Arnett R V Kruk 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1982,8(2):225-237
Each of four groups of 12 subjects performed four psychophysical tasks. The age ranges of the four groups were 19-31, 45-57, 58-70, and 71-83 years, respectively. All four tasks required some form of visual information processing: Two were backward-masking tasks; two were temporal-integration tasks. In all four tasks increasing temporal functions over age were obtained, suggesting slower processing rates as age increased. The results support an active processing model of visual perception that interprets duration of visible persistence and duration of interval in which backward masking is effective as indices of the time course of early stages in the processing of stimulus features. The evidences also suggests that backward masking and visible persistence may be mediated by distinct mechanisms that are affected differently by aging processes. A model that conceptualizes the visual system as a multichannel processor is proposed as an explanation for some of the findings. 相似文献
812.
V. J. Polidora 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1966,1(11):405-414
Fourteen monkeys were presented with a series of simultaneous discrimination problems, each problem consisting of two visual metric patterns, and Ss’ performance was analyzed with respect to several physical dimensions of the patterns. Proficiency of discriminative performance was found to be directly and almost exclusively related to the extent to which the two patterns, when translated, had unique parts. 相似文献
813.
Frederick V. Malmstrom Robert J. Randle John S. Bendix Robert J. Weber 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,28(5):440-448
Concurrent mental activity seems to be a significant, nonvisual factor affecting the human accommodation response. Two experiments were conducted to determine the direction and magnitude of this accommodation response. Experiment 1 employed a concurrent, written backwards counting task. Experiment 2 employed a concurrent, mental imagery task of “thinking near” and “thinking far.” In both experiments, the concurrent secondary task effected a cumulative accommodative shift toward the visual far point of from .25 to .75 diopter away from a near (3.0 diopter) target. This accommodative shift was observed only in the presence of a stimulus field and not in open-loop (analogous to empty-field) conditions. In addition, a long-term instability in the open-loop method of obtaining the dark focus was observed. Similarities between this accommodative shift and the pupillary response are noted. The accommodation response is discussed in relationship to both an attention-sharing and an involuntary autonomic response model. 相似文献
814.
815.
Surviving members of the Berkeley Older Generation Study were interviewed and tested with the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale in 1969-1970 and again in 1983-1984, when subjects' ages ranged from 73 to 93. Health was assessed by self-reports at both measurement periods. Although many individuals showed some decline in intellectual functioning, substantial individual differences were apparent at all age levels. More than one half of the subjects showed no reliable change, and a minority showed a reliable increase in verbal scores. The role of self-reported health has increasing importance in the maintenance of intellectual functioning in advanced old age. 相似文献
816.
There are increasing numbers of self-referral stress-management programs, a few of which use group-relaxation techniques, but few data are available on the personalities of the symptomatic and asymptomatic clients who attend or on any changes in personality after the program. Scores on the Eysenck Personality Inventory, Multiple Health Locus of Control, and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory as well as demographic information were obtained from 255 adults who attended a 10-session, university-based, group-relaxation program. The symptomatic clients reported significantly less anxiety, less neuroticism, were more extraverted and ascribed less of their behavior to chance at a 1 month post-treatment follow-up than at intake. The small group of asymptomatic clients also reported less anxiety and neuroticism at the follow-up. There were no sex differences on the Eysenck inventory or the anxiety scales but there were for the Multiple Health internal and control scales. It was concluded that group relaxation appeared to improve mental health scores effectively for both stressed and nonstressed adults and that age was significantly related to some personality scores. 相似文献
817.
818.
Choice reaction times following correct verbal predictions of the presented stimulus are faster than after incorrect predictions. Two experiments examined the influence of the time interval (PSI) between the prediction and the stimulus on the magnitude of the prediction effect. Experiment 1 used equiprobable stimuli and showed a smaller prediction effect at a 2-sec PSI than at a 0- or .5-sec PSI. Experiment 2 used unequal-probability stimuli and showed that the prediction effect was independent of PSI when the more frequent stimulus was predicted. When the less frequent stimulus was predicted, however, the magnitude of the prediction effect was smaller at .75- and 2-sec PSIs than at a 0-sec PSI. These results were discussed in terms of a prediction substitution hypothesis. 相似文献
819.
Jenö Putnoky 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1979,8(6):543-558
This article deals with relations between mental motor evoking capacity (motority, Putnoky, 1975a, b) and the sensory and affective connotations, as well as certain denotations, of words. Sensory connotation is indexed by degree of visual auditory, and tactual imagery, as assessed on a 7-point scale, while affective connotation is indexed by the usual three factors tapped by the semantic differential. The aspect of denotation used here is the abstractness value of words, also based on rating by scales. The general conclusions about motority are as follows: (1) the finding that its sensory connotations are mainly via theauditory modality of imagery, and especially in correlation with abstract words, suggests that it has a specific function that may have originated in the processing of heard language; (2) it correlates with thepotency andactivity factors of affective connotation, which suggests the existence of a nonspecific function or component in it; (3) correlations with abstractness reflect that motority must have a denotative function or component, especially in case of abstract verbal stimuli. On the basis of the results, motority is supposed to play a regulative role at higher levels of the organization of word meaning in particular.Computation of SD data was carried out under the direction and according to the programming of Professor William H. May in the Center for Comparative Psycholinguistics (Director: Charles E. Osgood), University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, with support provided by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH 07705) and National Science Foundation (NSF GS 360 and 2012-X). 相似文献
820.