首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   73737篇
  免费   3029篇
  国内免费   30篇
  2020年   830篇
  2019年   989篇
  2018年   1421篇
  2017年   1469篇
  2016年   1538篇
  2015年   1111篇
  2014年   1313篇
  2013年   6323篇
  2012年   2411篇
  2011年   2435篇
  2010年   1465篇
  2009年   1479篇
  2008年   2069篇
  2007年   2092篇
  2006年   1875篇
  2005年   1595篇
  2004年   1624篇
  2003年   1561篇
  2002年   1510篇
  2001年   2458篇
  2000年   2290篇
  1999年   1717篇
  1998年   799篇
  1997年   707篇
  1996年   793篇
  1995年   706篇
  1994年   685篇
  1993年   683篇
  1992年   1439篇
  1991年   1346篇
  1990年   1340篇
  1989年   1269篇
  1988年   1254篇
  1987年   1155篇
  1986年   1179篇
  1985年   1208篇
  1984年   1005篇
  1983年   856篇
  1979年   1052篇
  1978年   790篇
  1975年   853篇
  1974年   1002篇
  1973年   1024篇
  1972年   858篇
  1971年   804篇
  1970年   745篇
  1969年   698篇
  1968年   909篇
  1967年   805篇
  1966年   747篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
132.
133.
Abstract

This study builds on and extends previous sales leadership research by exploring sales professionals’ perceptions of effective leadership behaviors. Semistructured interviews with both sales leaders and salespeople working in a global enterprise software company were examined through a qualitative analysis. Results indicated that participants believed sales leadership played an important role in influencing sales performance. When asked to describe specific sales leader behaviors that best enable salesperson performance, sales professionals – both sales leaders and salespeople – overwhelmingly referenced coaching, followed by collaborating, championing, and customer engaging. We define and describe these four key sales leader behaviors and identify four potential mediating variables (trust, confidence, optimism, and resilience), from which emerges a conceptual framework of sales leader behaviors perceived to enable salesperson performance. We examine these four key sales leader behaviors and mediators in the broader context of leadership theory, particularly transformational, servant, authentic, and adaptive leadership theories. The key contribution of this study is the identification of a set of leader behaviors that are likely to be especially effective in modern sales organizations given that they originated from the perceptions of sales professionals themselves.  相似文献   
134.
135.
There is an apparent tension in our everyday moral responsibility practices. On the one hand, it is commonly assumed that moral responsibility requires voluntary control: an agent can be morally responsible only for those things that fall within the scope of her voluntary control. On the other hand, we regularly praise and blame individuals for mental states and conditions that appear to fall outside the scope of their voluntary control, such as desires, emotions, beliefs, and other attitudes. In order to resolve this apparent tension, many philosophers appeal to a tracing principle to argue that agents are morally responsible (only) for those attitudes whose existence can be traced back, causally, to a voluntary action or omission in the past. My aim in this article is to critically evaluate this tracing strategy and to argue that it gives us a misguided picture of when and why we are morally responsible for our attitudes. I argue that we should accept a ‘judgment sensitivity’ condition of moral responsibility rather than a ‘voluntary control’ condition, and defend this account against various objections.  相似文献   
136.
137.
138.
139.
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号