全文获取类型
收费全文 | 998篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
1023篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 16篇 |
2019年 | 15篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 29篇 |
2013年 | 107篇 |
2012年 | 50篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 35篇 |
2008年 | 64篇 |
2007年 | 49篇 |
2006年 | 46篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 38篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 38篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 15篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 6篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1023条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
171.
Horst Kächele Ulrike Oerter Nicola Scheytt-Hölzer Hans Ulrich Schmidt 《Psychotherapeut》2003,48(3):155-165
The authors report about the clinical implementation of music therapy in psychosomatic hospitals in FRG based on a survey among practising music therapists: attending especially to the qualification of music therapists, their integration and the problem of indication. The coverage of music therapy in the standard textbooks on psychosomatic medicine is discussed.Finally the state of research on music therapy in psychosomatic hospitals is reviewed. 相似文献
172.
Robert Nichols David R. Loy Nikky-Guninder Kaur Singh Carol Thirumaran Carl Olson N. Sreekumar M. Whitney Kelting Narasingha P. Sil Gereon Kopf M. Whitney Kelting John E. Cort Prabha C. Reddy Wayne Howard Deepak Sarma James B. Apple Steven E. Lindquist David Carpenter Carl Olson Carl Olson Ramakrishna Puligandla Hillary Rodrigues Katherine E. Ulrich Tamar Reich 《International Journal of Hindu Studies》2003,7(1-3):193-228
173.
174.
Roger E. Ulrich William C. Holz Nathan H. Azrin 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1964,7(2):129-133
The introduction of a warning signal preceding shocks greatly increased the effectiveness of avoidance responding. Periods of “warm-up” at the beginning of the session were eliminated, and the number of shocks received by the subjects was greatly reduced. With response-shock interval constant, response rate increased as the interval between the response and the onset of the warning signal was shortened. The response tended to occur shortly after the onset of the warning signal regardless of the duration of these “safe” periods. A greatly elevated response rate was maintained even when the duration of the safe period was reduced to 0.3 sec. Thus, the pre-shock signal obtained nearly exclusive control of the responding and overrode the usual “temporal discrimination” of the response-shock interval. 相似文献
175.
The disruptive behavior of a 9-yr-old boy was eliminated by the illumination of a light on the subject's desk, which represented the loss of free time later in the day. Instructions alone failed to reduce the frequency of disruptive behavior. When the light was removed and disruptive behavior no longer resulted in a loss of free time, disruptive behavior returned to its previous level. 相似文献
176.
Newcomer and veteran fraternity members indicated their own attitudinal position on each of 10 issues and then estimated the positions of either prospective or full in-group members or they received no opportunity to project. All subjects then rated their certainty about their own position on each issue. As expected. both newcomers and veterans assumed greater attitudinal similarity to in-group members than to prospective members. Moreover, projection onto in-group members only increased the opinion certainty of veteran members. Their certainty correlated with the percentage of their friends who were in-group members. Newcomers' certainty increased only after projection onto the lower status recruits. Membership tenure and social identity are discussed in relation to attitudinal projection and the goals of social comparison. 相似文献
177.
This paper links experimental psycholinguistics and theoretical syntax in the study of subject-verb agreement. Three experiments of elicited spoken production making use of specific characteristics of Italian and French are presented. They manipulate and examine its impact on the occurrence of 'attraction' errors (i.e. incorrect agreement with a word that is not the subject of the sentence). Experiment 1 (in Italian) shows that subject modifiers do not trigger attraction errors in free inverted VS (Verb Subject) structures, although attraction was found in VS interrogatives in English (Vigliocco, G., & Nicol, J. (1998). Separating hierarchical relations and word order in language production. Is proximity concord syntactic or linear? Cognition, 13-29) In Experiment 2 (in French), we report stronger attraction with preverbal clitic object pronouns than with subject modifiers. Experiment 3 (in French) shows that displaced direct objects in the cleft construction trigger attraction effects, in spite of the fact that the object does not intervene between the subject and the verb in the surface word order (OSV). Moreover, attraction is stronger in structures with subject-verb inversion (...). These observations are shown to be naturally interpretable through the tools of formal syntax, as elaborated within the Principles and Parameters/Minimalist tradition. Three important constructs are discussed: (1) the hierarchical representation of the sentence during syntactic construction, and the role of intermediate positions by which words transit when they move; (2) the role of specific hierarchical (c-command) but also linear (precedence) relations; and (3) the possibility that agreement involves two functionally distinct components. A gradient of computational complexity in agreement is presented which relates empirical evidence to these theoretical constructs. 相似文献
178.
179.
180.