全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1933篇 |
免费 | 115篇 |
专业分类
2048篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 35篇 |
2018年 | 61篇 |
2017年 | 44篇 |
2016年 | 51篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 41篇 |
2013年 | 215篇 |
2012年 | 77篇 |
2011年 | 68篇 |
2010年 | 51篇 |
2009年 | 38篇 |
2008年 | 91篇 |
2007年 | 81篇 |
2006年 | 72篇 |
2005年 | 47篇 |
2004年 | 43篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 52篇 |
1999年 | 46篇 |
1998年 | 27篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 30篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 37篇 |
1991年 | 21篇 |
1990年 | 23篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 20篇 |
1981年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 26篇 |
1977年 | 23篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1975年 | 23篇 |
1974年 | 18篇 |
1971年 | 17篇 |
1968年 | 23篇 |
1967年 | 15篇 |
1966年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有2048条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Raes F Hermans D Williams JM Demyttenaere K Sabbe B Pieters G Eelen P 《Memory (Hove, England)》2006,14(5):584-594
The present study explored the relation between overgeneral autobiographical memory (AM) and other aspects of memory functioning in depression. A total of 26 patients with major depressive disorder completed a set of memory tasks measuring AM specificity (AMT; Williams & Broadbent, 1986), working memory, semantic memory, verbal learning, delayed verbal recall, recognition memory, and source memory. Reduced specificity of AM was related to poor working memory (central executive functioning) and poor source memory. The former finding conforms to the idea that the voluntary retrieval of specific autobiographical memories (AMs) involves central executive processes (e.g., Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). The latter finding replicates and extends recent findings suggesting that overgeneral AM is part of a broader memory deficit in retrieving the specific details of the context in which information was acquired (Ramponi, Barnard, & Nimmo-Smith, 2004). Furthermore, in line with Ramponi et al. (2004), rumination was found to be related to both overgeneral AM and poor source memory. 相似文献
73.
A wide range of abilities was assessed in 56 high-functioning children with autism and 56 age- and IQ-matched controls. Stepwise discriminant analyses produced good group discrimination for sensory-perceptual, motor, complex language, and complex memory domains but lower agreement for the reasoning domain than previously obtained for adults. Group discrimination did not occur for attention, simple language, simple memory, and visuospatial domains. Findings provide additional support for a complex information-processing model for autism, previously based on adult data, demonstrating a pattern across domains of selective impairments on measures with high demands for integration of information and sparing when demands were low. Children as compared to adults with autism exhibited more prominent sensory-perceptual symptoms and less pronounced reasoning deficits reflecting brain maturation. 相似文献
74.
Williams MT Schaefer TL Furay AR Ehrman LA Vorhees CV 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》2006,9(3):153-163
We examined the ontogeny of the corticosterone response to (+)-methamphetamine in neonatal rats. In experiment-1, animals were injected with 10 mg/kg of (+)-methamphetamine or saline and plasma corticosterone levels were examined in separate groups 30 or 105 min later on postnatal day (P) 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 17, or 19. The adrenal response to methamphetamine was best described by a U-shaped function with the nadir of corticosterone release occurring between P7 and P13. Experiment-2 was similar except that the effect of four consecutive days of exposure to (+)-methamphetamine (four times daily at 2 h intervals with 10 mg/kg) was assessed with a single final dose early on the fifth day (i.e. P1-5, 3-7, 5-9, 7-11, 9-13, 11-15, 13-17, 15-19). The 30 min corticosterone response after multiple methamphetamine doses was augmented compared to single exposures, with the exception of the two earliest dosing intervals ending on P5 and P7, where the responses were lower. In addition, at 105 min, the levels of corticosterone were attenuated relative to a single drug administration. With the exception of animals receiving methamphetamine from P15 to P19, thymus weights were unaffected. The data demonstrate that (+)-methamphetamine is a robust activator of corticosterone release in developing animals and this release is extensively modified by age and previous drug exposure. 相似文献
75.
John Nicholas Williams 《Philosophical Studies》2006,127(3):383-414
For Moore, it is a paradox that although I would be absurd in asserting that (it is raining but I don’t believe it is) or
that (it is raining but I believe it isn’t), such assertions might be true. But I would be also absurd in judging that the
contents of such assertions are true. I argue for the strategy of explaining the absurdity of Moorean assertion in terms of
conscious Moorean belief. Only in this way may the pathology of Moorean absurdity be adequately explained in terms of self-contradiction.
David Rosenthal disagrees with this strategy. Ironically, his higher-order thought account has the resources to fulfil it.
Indeed once modified and supplemented, it compares favourably with Brentano’s rival account of conscious belief.
*This paper was written with the support of a grant from the SMU-Wharton Research center. 相似文献
76.
Linwood G. Vereen S. Kent Butler Franklyn C. Williams Jules A. Darg Trae K. E. Downing 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2006,84(1):10-15
According to the literature, humor is a critical tool to enhance the counseling process because it can reduce stress, build rapport, and aid in the increase of the client's self‐efficacy. In recent years, the critical nature of using humor appropriately from a cultural perspective has been explored. In this article, the authors further explore the cultural foundations, purpose, effectiveness, and implications of using humor when counseling African American college students. 相似文献
77.
D. G. Williams 《Personality and individual differences》1981,2(4):303-309
The scores of 33 subjects of the neuroticism scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire correlated more highly with mood levels averaged over the 12 occasions studied than, on average, with mood scores on single occasions, and more highly with mood described in global terms than with scores on each of the original 18 mood scales. Subjects with high neuroticism scores had poorer mood on average and showed greater variation in mood pattern from one occasion to another than did more stable subjects. It is suggested that forms of relatively poor mood may be more varied than forms of good mood, and that, although neuroticism makes a poor mood more likely, the precise composition of this mood on any occasion may be determined by the combination of situation characteristics and person variables. Psychoticism scores correlated significantly, though weakly, with the average scores for some mood scales independently of neuroticism. 相似文献
78.
Pigeons were trained on a multiple schedule in which separate concurrent schedules were presented in the two components of the schedule. During one component, concurrent variable-interval 40-sec variable-interval 80-sec schedules operated. In the second component, concurrent variable-interval 40-sec variable-interval 20-sec schedules operated. After stable baseline performance was obtained in both components, extinction probe choice tests were presented to assess preference between the variable-interval 40-sec schedules from the two components. The variable-interval 40-sec schedule paired with the variable-interval 80-sec schedule was preferred over the variable-interval 40-sec schedule paired with the variable-interval 20-sec schedule. The subjects were also exposed to several resistance-to-change manipulations: (1) prefeeding prior to the experimental session, (2) a free-food schedule added to timeout periods separating components, and (3) extinction. The results indicated that preference and resistance to change do not necessarily covary. 相似文献
79.
80.
Sara‐Jayne Williams Daniel B. Wright Norman H. Freeman 《Applied cognitive psychology》2002,16(6):651-664
Children are generally more susceptible than adults to suggestive interview techniques. Children's memories of an event can be altered and added to by presenting post‐event information (PEI). What is not known is whether embedding silence about a particular scene within the PEI makes that scene less likely to be reported. Children aged 5–6 years made cakes with an agent ‘Mrs Flour’. The following day they received PEI in which a target scene from the original event was omitted, resulting in children reporting the target scene significantly less often than did controls (control= 57% and omit= 23% correct responses). There was direct evidence from the children's language that the omission led to a detriment in memory for the original scene itself. Allowing children to draw during the interview did not reduce the effect. Implications are discussed in terms of child victims and witnesses particularly regarding child sexual abuse. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献