全文获取类型
收费全文 | 25597篇 |
免费 | 482篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
26081篇 |
出版年
2018年 | 338篇 |
2017年 | 363篇 |
2016年 | 370篇 |
2015年 | 266篇 |
2014年 | 352篇 |
2013年 | 1800篇 |
2012年 | 593篇 |
2011年 | 646篇 |
2010年 | 443篇 |
2009年 | 403篇 |
2008年 | 623篇 |
2007年 | 561篇 |
2006年 | 555篇 |
2005年 | 465篇 |
2004年 | 472篇 |
2003年 | 472篇 |
2002年 | 483篇 |
2001年 | 649篇 |
2000年 | 648篇 |
1999年 | 505篇 |
1998年 | 260篇 |
1997年 | 278篇 |
1996年 | 273篇 |
1995年 | 263篇 |
1992年 | 494篇 |
1991年 | 458篇 |
1990年 | 446篇 |
1989年 | 422篇 |
1988年 | 443篇 |
1987年 | 410篇 |
1986年 | 452篇 |
1985年 | 431篇 |
1984年 | 382篇 |
1983年 | 338篇 |
1981年 | 272篇 |
1979年 | 408篇 |
1978年 | 329篇 |
1977年 | 284篇 |
1976年 | 297篇 |
1975年 | 345篇 |
1974年 | 436篇 |
1973年 | 464篇 |
1972年 | 343篇 |
1971年 | 369篇 |
1970年 | 337篇 |
1969年 | 364篇 |
1968年 | 440篇 |
1967年 | 402篇 |
1966年 | 418篇 |
1958年 | 253篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
A detailed study of sequential saccadic eye movements for normal- and poor-reading children 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
This paper presents the results of an in-depth study of parameters characterizing sequenced saccadic eye movements for a group of dyslexic children and a comparative normal control group with ages in the range greater than 8.0 yr. and less than 13.0 yr. No parameters were statistically different for the two groups, which supports the findings of Brown, et al. and contradicts the findings of Pavlidis. Our results indicate that sequenced saccadic eye movements are not diagnostically useful for early detection of dyslexia. 相似文献
192.
Previous research aimed at quantifying and reducing the decrements encountered in performing manual tasks in cold weather has not described the relationships between task characteristics and cold-induced impairments. The amount of decrement and, often times, the optimal means for reducing the decrement, appear to be task-specific. The research reported here is one in a series of studies formulated to explore those relationships. The strategy is to use tasks from the battery developed by Fleishman (1967) to measure the factorially 'pure' abilities needed to perform all manual tasks. 24 U.S. Marines performed this battery of nine tasks across a range of cold temperatures. To determine if the decrement due to wearing gloves might be less than the decrement due to cold hands as the air temperature decreased, performance on the battery of tasks was measured with and without gloves. Only three of the tasks (abilities) were affected by cold temperatures, and the amount of decrement increased as the air temperature decreased. Three tasks deteriorated due to wearing of gloves, two of those affected by cold and one other. Temperature affected performance independently of the glove effect. Half of the subjects did not complete bare-handed testing at -18 degrees C (approximately 20 min.), indicating this is the lower end of the temperature range in which bare-handed performance for more than a few minutes is practical. 相似文献
193.
T Krause W P Pankau A Wagner J L?ssner 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1984,36(9):545-550
By 48 patients with Wilson's disease the brainstem acoustic evoked potentials were stated. There were 35 pathological findings (73%), 13 were normal (27%). The waves, determined by the middle and upper brainstem (Pons and Mesencephalon) showed most of all pathological changes. Those patients with forms of Wilson's disease called Pseudoskelerose and Pseudoparkinson showed the most pathological findings. But one could watch these findings by about 30% of patients in preclinical stage. This method can possibly be used for early detection of disorders of brainstem functions and in the same way it can be used for control of treatment by patients in preclinical stage. 相似文献
194.
195.
196.
197.
The odd-even status of a sum depends on the odd-even status of its addends. A sum must be odd if an odd number of its addends are odd; else it must be even. A proposed sum that violates the required odd-even status of the sum—that is, deviates from the correct sum, whether odd or even, by an odd value (e.g., splits of ±1, ±3, ±5)—can be rejected immediately as false. Subjects in the present study did indeed use the odd-even rule in sum verification, because they were as fast and accurate in rejecting a split of ±1 as one of ±2, and a split of ±3 as one of ±4, even though a larger split generally is easier to reject (symbolic distance effect), and splits of ±3 and ±4 were rejected faster and more accurately than those of ±1 and ±2. Performance on separate odd-even tasks indicated that the odd-even properties of numbers and sums are readily available for use by adults, and that persons who do well on such tasks are especially likely to use the odd-even rule in sum verification. 相似文献
198.
199.
200.
Children were exposed to a multiple schedule involving equal variable-interval schedules in each of two components and a multiple schedule involving a variable-interval schedule in one component and an extinction schedule in the other. Response rates were equal in both components when each involved a variable-interval schedule. Response rates differed in the two components of the multiple variable-interval extinction schedule. Response rates were higher in the variable-interval schedule when the accompanying schedule was extinction than when it was variable interval. The increase in response rate in the variable-interval component, simultaneous with the decrease in response rate in the extinction component, illustrated sustained behavioral contrast, and was the first evidence of this phenomenon in children. 相似文献