首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   296篇
  免费   13篇
  309篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   6篇
  2020年   7篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   16篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   19篇
  2015年   13篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   52篇
  2012年   11篇
  2011年   11篇
  2010年   17篇
  2009年   16篇
  2008年   17篇
  2007年   11篇
  2006年   4篇
  2005年   7篇
  2004年   5篇
  2003年   5篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   1篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   4篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   4篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   1篇
  1984年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   5篇
  1980年   1篇
  1978年   2篇
  1976年   1篇
  1975年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1971年   2篇
  1970年   2篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   1篇
  1965年   2篇
  1964年   2篇
  1963年   1篇
  1956年   1篇
排序方式: 共有309条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
Popular images and stereotypes of women in the menopausal age range are overwhelmingly negative. Because these stereotypes are likely both to influence and to be influenced by published scholarship, it is particularly important to examine conventional knowledge as it has been represented in science-based journals. In an effort to examine the extent and nature of the accepted knowledge base regarding menopause, a survey of both medical and psychological journal articles was conducted for the years 1984–1994. Publication trends revealed a predominance of articles based on a biomedical paradigm and the virtual absence of articles presenting alternative perspectives on midlife. Ten serious methodological problems common to this literature are delineated, including such fundamental errors as failure to acquire baseline data, lack of control groups, vague operational definitions, and blatantly pejorative language. We also discuss conceptual flaws implicit in the predominant paradigm, including the messages that women are different, sicker, and weaker than a normal, male, ideal. Finally, we consider the implications of these social constructions for the political, social, and psychological status of women.  相似文献   
192.
193.
    
An increasing number of researchers are exploring variations of the Concealed Knowledge Test (CKT) as alternatives to traditional ‘lie‐detector’ tests. For example, the response times (RT)‐based CKT has been previously shown to accurately detect participants who possess privileged knowledge. Although several studies have reported successful RT‐based tests, they have focused on verbal stimuli despite the prevalence of photographic evidence in forensic investigations. Related studies comparing pictures and phrases have yielded inconsistent results. The present work compared an RT‐CKT using verbal phrases as stimuli to one using pictures of faces. This led to equally accurate and efficient tests using either stimulus type. Results also suggest that previous inconsistent findings may be attributable to study procedures that led to better memory for verbal than visual items. When memory for verbal phrases and pictures were equated, we found nearly identical detection accuracies. Copyright © 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
194.
    
Effective administrators understand and use such basic financial concepts as resource allocation in a systems context, development stages of an agency, and guidelines for using financial and accounting information for decisions regarding venture capital, fee income, and cost recovery.  相似文献   
195.
    
Because Program evaluation entails both financial and nonfinancial information, a rationale for jnancial analysis is presented to help administrators combine conclusions with the value judgments that are inescapably interrelated.  相似文献   
196.
Can one be fooled into believing that one intended an action that one in fact did not intend? Past experimental paradigms have demonstrated that participants, when provided with false perceptual feedback about their actions, can be fooled into misperceiving the nature of their intended motor act. However, because veridical proprioceptive/perceptual feedback limits the extent to which participants can be fooled, few studies have been able to answer our question and induce the illusion to intend. In a novel paradigm addressing this question, participants were instructed to move a line on the computer screen by use of a phony brain–computer interface. Line movements were actually controlled by computer program. Demonstrating the illusion to intend, participants reported more intentions to move the line when it moved frequently than when it moved infrequently. Consistent with ideomotor theory, the finding illuminates the intimate liaisons among ideomotor processing, the sense of agency, and action production.  相似文献   
197.
198.
199.
Anger is a powerful emotion prevalent in therapy. An integrative model situating varied manifestations of anger within a purposive context of healing and repair following relationship trauma is needed. A first paper (Butler, Meloy-Miller, Seedall, & Dicus, 2017) approached anger as a psychological and relational construction of diffuse physiological arousal and provided a conceptual model of how the experience of offense interacts with a person's view of self in relation to other (VSIRO) in the formation of anger. Three resulting trajectories and pathways of anger were outlined. Here we provide clinical representations of these pathways—two pathways of hostile anger arising from pathogenic VSIROs (inflated, with accompanying externalizing anger, and collapsed, with accompanying internalizing anger), and a third pathway of benevolent anger, arising from a balanced VSIRO. Clinical application of the model through several vignettes illuminates its use in discriminating helpful from harmful pathways of anger and developing interventions for reshaping pathogenic anger to beneficent anger.  相似文献   
200.
Neuropsychology Review - Mindfulness-based programs (MBPs) are increasingly utilized to improve mental health. Interest in the putative effects of MBPs on cognitive function is also growing. This...  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号