全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3948篇 |
免费 | 209篇 |
专业分类
4157篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 39篇 |
2022年 | 43篇 |
2021年 | 75篇 |
2020年 | 87篇 |
2019年 | 119篇 |
2018年 | 155篇 |
2017年 | 155篇 |
2016年 | 171篇 |
2015年 | 123篇 |
2014年 | 144篇 |
2013年 | 484篇 |
2012年 | 214篇 |
2011年 | 240篇 |
2010年 | 141篇 |
2009年 | 145篇 |
2008年 | 196篇 |
2007年 | 159篇 |
2006年 | 135篇 |
2005年 | 106篇 |
2004年 | 127篇 |
2003年 | 131篇 |
2002年 | 102篇 |
2001年 | 77篇 |
2000年 | 87篇 |
1999年 | 58篇 |
1998年 | 41篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 22篇 |
1995年 | 25篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 14篇 |
1992年 | 38篇 |
1991年 | 26篇 |
1990年 | 45篇 |
1989年 | 44篇 |
1988年 | 39篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 31篇 |
1985年 | 30篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 20篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1938年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有4157条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
62.
Using a meta‐analytical procedure, the relationship between team composition in terms of the Big‐Five personality traits (trait elevation and variability) and team performance were researched. The number of teams upon which analyses were performed ranged from 106 to 527. For the total sample, significant effects were found for elevation in agreeableness (ρ = 0.24) and conscientiousness (ρ = 0.20), and for variability in agreeableness (ρ = ?0.12) and conscientiousness (ρ = ?0.24). Moderation by type of team was tested for professional teams versus student teams. Moderation results for agreeableness and conscientiousness were in line with the total sample results. However, student and professional teams differed in effects for emotional stability and openness to experience. Based on these results, suggestions for future team composition research are presented. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
63.
Bas C. van Fraassen 《Topoi》2006,25(1-2):123-127
Looking back from 2049 over one-hundred and fifty years of philosophy, a student's essay reveals what became of rival strands in Western philosophy – with a sidelong glance at the special Topoi issue on the theme “Philosophy: What is to be Done?” that was published almost half a century earlier. 相似文献
64.
Paul L. Harris Arne Kruithof Mark Meerum Terwogt Ton Visser 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1981,31(2):212-230
Children of 8 and 11 years were assessed in two experiments for their sensitivity to textual anomaly. In Experiment 1, subjects read stories containing two target lines, one appropriate and the other anomalous in relation to previously given information. Both age groups read the anomalous line more slowly than the appropriate line, but in a subsequent test of comprehension monitoring, the older group was more likely than the younger group to pick out the anomalous line as not fitting in with the rest of the story. Experiment 2 produced similar results: both 8- and 11-year-old children read an anomalous line more slowly, but 11-year-olds were more likely than 8-year-olds to cite the anomalous line or part of it when questioned about the possible presence of a line that did not fit in with the rest of the story. The results indicate that an age change in comprehension monitoring as indexed by citation or selection of a textual anomaly need not be contingent upon a parallel age change in constructive processing as indexed by modulation of reading rate. 相似文献
65.
A linear utility model is introduced for optimal selection when several subpopulations of applicants are to be distinguished. Using this model, procedures are described for obtaining optimal cutting scores in subpopulations in quota-free as well as quota-restricted selection situations. The cutting scores are optimal in the sense that they maximize the overall expected utility of the selection process. The procedures are demonstrated with empirical data. 相似文献
66.
Henk G Schmidt Henny P A Boshuizen Gerard J P van Breukelen 《Memory (Hove, England)》2002,10(1):21-28
Four actors were requested to perform Sartre's No Exit after a retention interval of more than 5 months. Their recall of the play was studied either with the spatial and visual contextual cues normally available during a performance or without such cues. Total recall was still considerable, equalling 85%. The number of paraphrases of, and inferences on, the original text was however quite large (32%), suggesting that the actors had learned their lines according to their meaning rather than as a result of rote memorisation. The context manipulation showed that actors' recall is facilitated by the availability of spatial-visual information, but only to a limited extent. The relative importance of textual and contextual cues in the long-term retention of a play is discussed. 相似文献
67.
M. J. H. van Onna 《Psychometrika》2002,67(4):519-538
In a latent class IRT model in which the latent classes are ordered on one dimension, the class specific response probabilities are subject to inequality constraints. The number of these inequality constraints increase dramatically with the number of response categories per item, if assumptions like monotonicity or double monotonicity of the cumulative category response functions are postulated. A Markov chain Monte Carlo method, the Gibbs sampler, can sample from the multivariate posterior distribution of the parameters under the constraints. Bayesian model selection can be done by posterior predictive checks and Bayes factors. A simulation study is done to evaluate results of the application of these methods to ordered latent class models in three realistic situations. Also, an example of the presented methods is given for existing data with polytomous items. It can be concluded that the Bayesian estimation procedure can handle the inequality constraints on the parameters very well. However, the application of Bayesian model selection methods requires more research. 相似文献
68.
69.
We examine how group members paradoxically refuse intergroup help where they might need it most: in the moral status domain. Based on the Sacred Value Protection Model (Tetlock, 2002), we predicted and found that group members felt stronger group-based anger and a stronger motivation to reaffirm their group's moral status when an outgroup was morally superior to them. Despite this moral motivation, however, we also predicted and found that group members more strongly refused intergroup help to improve their moral status vis-à-vis the morally superior outgroup (compared to an uninvolved outgroup). Consistent with the Social Identity model of Deindividuation Effects (Reicher, Spears, & Postmes, 1995), group members thus strategically refused intergroup help to defend their group identity. Supporting this interpretation, particularly highly identified group members were most likely to refuse intergroup help when they needed it most. We discuss theoretical and practical implications of our findings. 相似文献
70.
The present research examines the role of optimism on time preferences for both losses and gains. It is argued that optimism has asymmetric effects on time preferences for gains versus losses: one reason why decision makers prefer immediate gains is because they are optimistic that these gains will be followed by additional gains in future. In contrast, decision makers prefer to delay losses because they are optimistic that losses are avoidable in the future. Optimism about outcomes affects time preferences for both gains and losses, such that low optimism reduces the discount rates while increased optimism is associated with higher discount rates. This prediction was supported in two different domains: monetary outcomes (Study 1), and health (Study 2). Implications of these results for both research practice and time preferences in the real world are discussed. 相似文献