全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1131篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
1157篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 19篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 31篇 |
2018年 | 39篇 |
2017年 | 52篇 |
2016年 | 49篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 31篇 |
2013年 | 123篇 |
2012年 | 61篇 |
2011年 | 77篇 |
2010年 | 38篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 41篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 39篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 39篇 |
2003年 | 39篇 |
2002年 | 38篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 13篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有1157条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
921.
Two key research issues in the field of causal learning are how people acquire causal knowledge when observing data that are presented sequentially, and the level of abstraction at which learning takes place. Does sequential causal learning solely involve the acquisition of specific cause‐effect links, or do learners also acquire knowledge about abstract causal constraints? Recent empirical studies have revealed that experience with one set of causal cues can dramatically alter subsequent learning and performance with entirely different cues, suggesting that learning involves abstract transfer, and such transfer effects involve sequential presentation of distinct sets of causal cues. It has been demonstrated that pre‐training (or even post‐training) can modulate classic causal learning phenomena such as forward and backward blocking. To account for these effects, we propose a Bayesian theory of sequential causal learning. The theory assumes that humans are able to consider and use several alternative causal generative models, each instantiating a different causal integration rule. Model selection is used to decide which integration rule to use in a given learning environment in order to infer causal knowledge from sequential data. Detailed computer simulations demonstrate that humans rely on the abstract characteristics of outcome variables (e.g., binary vs. continuous) to select a causal integration rule, which in turn alters causal learning in a variety of blocking and overshadowing paradigms. When the nature of the outcome variable is ambiguous, humans select the model that yields the best fit with the recent environment, and then apply it to subsequent learning tasks. Based on sequential patterns of cue‐outcome co‐occurrence, the theory can account for a range of phenomena in sequential causal learning, including various blocking effects, primacy effects in some experimental conditions, and apparently abstract transfer of causal knowledge. 相似文献
922.
Only a handful of studies have extended our understanding of retrieval induced forgetting (RIF) during development and even fewer have tested for RIF effects outside word‐list paradigms. The purpose of these experiments was to: (i) examine how partial retrieval of a witnessed event would impact subsequent retrieval of that event in school‐aged children; and (ii) examine the robustness of semantic integration as a boundary condition on RIF. Two experiments were conducted using the three traditional phases of the RIF paradigm: study phase, practice phase, and test phase. We found clear evidence of RIF in event memory. There was also evidence of the robust impact that integration instructions have on minimizing RIF. Integration appears to not only have a dampening effect on RIF, but integration instructions may also influence how children process all aspects of an experience, regardless of whether a person is passively or actively part of the experience.Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
923.
924.
Tom Roberts 《Ratio》2014,27(2):190-204
What is it that unites episodes of the emotion of annoyance? The paper considers possible analyses of the content of the state of annoyance, and concludes that this emotion should be understood to involve a negative construal of an object, event, or state of affairs as having failed to exemplify one of a suite of kinds of everyday quality or excellence. This account permits us to see what is common to a varied range of superficially‐disjointed emotional responses, and to make sense of the conditions under which annoyance is appropriate or inappropriate. Moreover, it reveals something of what we care about in everyday and social contexts, in our ordinary dealings with persons and artefacts. 相似文献
925.
Tom Strong 《Journal of Family Therapy》2014,36(1):81-102
In this article we review Johnella Bird's notion of relational consciousness, explaining it in terms of an ethnomethodologically informed social constructionist theory. We extend this notion to her conversational practices in therapy, examining first her general practice (and focus) on relational language‐making. We then turn to describing three of her specific conversational practices – negotiating conflicting discursive positions between partners in a relationship, exploring a partner's experience of hurt in a relationship, following unspoken assumptions, and negotiating power relations. We conclude by relating relational consciousness to an attendance to language as it is used by clients and by therapists in dialogue with clients. 相似文献
926.
Tom F. Price Eddie Harmon‐Jones 《Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science》2015,6(6):461-473
A growing body of evidence suggests that certain facial expressions and postures are associated with emotional and motivational responses. This review discusses behavioral, neuroscientific, and cognitive research connecting these bodily movements with emotive responses. General bodily feedback theories of emotion have suggested that manipulated facial expressions and postures influence emotive reactions to stimuli as well as physiological responses such as heart rate, skin conductance, and the temperature of blood entering the brain. More recent evidence suggests that manipulated bodily states influence prefrontal cortical activation and amygdala activation. Even further evidence has suggested that manipulated bodily states influence cognitive processes, such as the speed at which individuals read emotional content, the speed at which they classify information as emotional, and the extent to which they determine emotional information as threatening. Bodily feedback theories may also suggest clinical applications. Bodily feedback theories of emotion therefore have generated research showing that bodily expressions play a pivotal role in our emotive experiences. WIREs Cogn Sci 2015, 6:461–473. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1370 This article is categorized under:
- Psychology > Emotion and Motivation
927.
Tom Angier 《European Journal of Philosophy》2014,22(4):540-572
I argue that, in analysing the structure and development of moral traditions, MacIntyre relies primarily on Kuhn's model of scientific tradition, rather than (as is held by at least two influential commentators) on Lakatos' model. I unpack three foci of Kuhn's conception of the sciences, namely: the ‘crisis’ conception of scientific development, what I call the ‘systematic conception’ of scientific paradigms, and the view that successive paradigms are incommensurable. I then show that these three foci are integrated into MacIntyre's account of the development of moral traditions with a surprising degree of faithfulness to Kuhn. And crucially, I argue against the overall cogency of his account, given the disparities I pinpoint between scientific and moral traditions. My overall critique is, however, fundamentally friendly, since nothing I have to say invalidates the very notion of a moral tradition, and all I am calling for are less problematic construals of that notion. 相似文献
928.
The invention of the computer has revolutionized science. With respect to finding the essential structures of life, for example,
it has enabled scientists not only to investigate empirical examples, but also to create and study novel hypothetical variations
by means of simulation: ‘life as it could be’. We argue that this kind of research in the field of artificial life, namely
the specification, implementation and evaluation of artificial systems, is akin to Husserl’s method of free imaginative variation
as applied to the specific regional ontology of biology. Thus, at a time when the clarification of the essence of our biological
embodiment is of growing interest for phenomenology, we suggest that artificial life should be seen as a method of externalizing
some of the insurmountable complexity of imaginatively varying the phenomenon of life. 相似文献
929.
Systematic studies of artistic careers are scarce and this is the first large-scale study on the career development of pop musicians. Using a prospective longitudinal approach we followed a sample of aspiring pop musicians in the Netherlands (N = 369) over a three-year period. First we identified four groups of pop musicians with different career patterns, that is, upward careers, downward careers, stable successful careers and stable unsuccessful careers. By means of a multigroup growth mixture model we examined how career success was influenced by social support, professional attitude and professional network. Results showed that successful pop musicians experience more social support, have a stronger professional attitude and a more extensive professional network. Moreover, our study shows that these findings were consistent both between and within the different career pattern groups. 相似文献
930.
It is commonly assumed that there is an interaction between the representations of number and space (e.g., Dehaene et al., 1993, Walsh, 2003), typically ascribed to a mental number line. The exact nature of this interaction has remained elusive, however. Here we propose that spatial aspects are not inherent to number representations, but that instead spatial and numerical representations are separate. However, cultural factors establish ties between them. By extending earlier models (Gevers et al., 2006, Verguts et al., 2005) based on this hypothesis, the authors present computer simulations showing that a model incorporating this idea can account for data from a series of studies. These results suggest that number–space interactions are emergent properties resulting from the interaction between different brain areas. 相似文献