首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   172877篇
  免费   7785篇
  国内免费   165篇
  180827篇
  2021年   1503篇
  2020年   2774篇
  2019年   3439篇
  2018年   3534篇
  2017年   3945篇
  2016年   4614篇
  2015年   3923篇
  2014年   4791篇
  2013年   23423篇
  2012年   4596篇
  2011年   3692篇
  2010年   3870篇
  2009年   4759篇
  2008年   3883篇
  2007年   3413篇
  2006年   4008篇
  2005年   3951篇
  2004年   3492篇
  2003年   3126篇
  2002年   2920篇
  2001年   3361篇
  2000年   3225篇
  1999年   3197篇
  1998年   2813篇
  1997年   2648篇
  1996年   2575篇
  1995年   2421篇
  1994年   2379篇
  1993年   2337篇
  1992年   2625篇
  1991年   2483篇
  1990年   2294篇
  1989年   2244篇
  1988年   2206篇
  1987年   2187篇
  1986年   2151篇
  1985年   2407篇
  1984年   2506篇
  1983年   2300篇
  1982年   2366篇
  1981年   2326篇
  1980年   2173篇
  1979年   2205篇
  1978年   2151篇
  1977年   2138篇
  1976年   1947篇
  1975年   1972篇
  1974年   2062篇
  1973年   1911篇
  1972年   1537篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
131.
132.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
133.
134.
135.
136.
137.
Shaw  Jamie 《Synthese》2021,198(1):419-449

To anyone vaguely aware of Feyerabend, the title of this paper would appear as an oxymoron. For Feyerabend, it is often thought, science is an anarchic practice with no discernible structure. Against this trend, I elaborate the groundwork that Feyerabend has provided for the beginnings of an approach to organizing scientific research. Specifically, I argue that Feyerabend’s pluralism, once suitably modified, provides a plausible account of how to organize science. These modifications come from C.S. Peirce’s account of the economics of theory pursuit, which has since been corroborated by empirical findings in the social sciences. I go on to contrast this approach with the conception of a ‘well-ordered science’ as outlined by Kitcher (Science, truth, and democracy, Oxford University Press, New York, 2001), Cartwright (Philos Sci 73(5):981–990, 2006), which rests on the assumption that we can predict the content of future research. I show how Feyerabend has already given us reasons to think that this model is much more limited than it is usually understood. I conclude by showing how models of resource allocation, specifically those of Kitcher (J Philos 87:5–22, 1990), Strevens (J Philos 100(2):55–79, 2003) and Weisberg and Muldoon (Philos Sci 76(2):225–252, 2009), unwittingly make use of this problematic assumption. I conclude by outlining a proposed model of resource allocation where funding is determined by lottery and briefly examining the extent to which it is compatible with the position defended in this paper.

  相似文献   
138.
An electronic apparatus using infrared beams for monitoring the movements of individual bees under dark conditions is described. The searching behavior of workers in an arena was monitored over 2-h periods. Mean ambulatory velocity for one bee over a distance of 100 mm was 45.6±1.51 mm/sec (n=45). Thigmokinesis and temporal activity patterns are illustrated.  相似文献   
139.
140.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号